considered169pp

169pp com  时间:2021-03-03  阅读:()
PARTIBiologicalandGeneticProcessesCHAPTERONEBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment:AReviewofRecentLiteratureJosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
BardIntroductionBehaviorgeneticsisaquantitativemethod,andadolescentdevelopmentisapsychologi-caltopic.
Treatingthecrossbetweenthesetwoarenasappears,atthesurface,torequirecollectingresearchinwhichthemethodhasbeenappliedtostudythetopic,andreview-ingthatresearchforcoherenceandcommonthemes.
Butthechallengeisrathermoredifcultthanthesurfacelevelviewmightsuggest.
Belowthesurfaceisagreatdealofshiftingsand,whichmakesorganizingthetopicdifcult.
Becauseofthisinstability,itiscriticalthatwecarefullyandexplicitlydeneafoundationalstartingpoint.
Intheintroductiontothisarticle,webeginwithsomedenitions,andthenwedescribethedifcultiesinherentinreviewing"behaviorgeneticsandadolescentdevelopment.
"Weconcludeourintroductionwithasummaryofthefoundationonwhichwewillbaseourreview.
Inthenextsection,wecarefullybuildthatfoundation.
Following,wesummarizetherelevantresearch,andembeditwithintheorganizationalfoundation.
Denitions:WhyIsThisChapterDifculttoFrameThestartingpointformostbehaviorgeneticmodelingistheconceptualpartitioningofsourcesofvarianceintogenetic,sharedenvironmental,andnonsharedenvironmental(e.
g.
,Rowe&Plomin,1981).
Someofthesimilaritybetweenindividualsinthesamefamilymaybecausedbysharingthesamegenes.
Forexample,monozygotic(MZ)twinsshare100percentoftheirgenes,andshouldbeapproximatelyidenticalontraitsorbehav-iorsthatarestronglyundergeneticcontrol(e.
g.
,height,eyecolor).
Dizygotic(DZ)twinsandfullsiblingsonaverageshare.
50oftheirgenes,halfsiblingsshare.
25,cousinsshare.
125,etc.
Alternatively,someofthesimilaritybetweenindividualsinthesamefamilyhasasitsetiologicalsourcethesharedenvironment;thistheoreticalsourceofinuencecausesrelatedindividualstobesimilarbecausetheyshareacommonenvironmentwithinthefamily.
Forexample,parentaldisciplinestylemaybeasharedenvironmentalinuencethatisincommontoallchildreninafamilyandthatresultsinsimilaritiesbetweenthechildreninameasureofresponsetoauthority.
Finally,thenonsharedenvironmentisatheoreticalsourceofinuencethatcausesindividualsinthesamefamilytobedifferentfromoneanother,thatis,theseinuencesarenotsharedincommonamongsiblings.
Forexample,aparticularlyoutstandingteacherthatachildhadinrstgrade(butwhowasnottheteacheroftheotherchildreninthefamily)couldcreatenonsharedenvironmen-talinuencesonreadingmotivation.
Or,evenwithinthefamily,parentsmaybemoreauthoritativewiththeirsonsthanwiththeirdaughters(whichwouldresultinparentaldisciplinestylebeinganonsharedratherthansharedenvironmentalinuence).
Itiswellknowntobehaviorgeneticists–ifnotnecessarilytothegeneralpopulationofresearchers–thatmeasuresofvariancerelatedtogeneticprocesses(heritabilities,orh2)andsharedenvironmentalprocesses(sometimescalledthecommonenvironment,orc2)arenotimmutable(seeAngoff,1988).
Thebasicbiometricalmodel(e.
g.
,Falconer,1981)partitionstheoverallvarianceinthedependentvariableintothatattributabletogenes,thatattributabletothesharedenvironment,andthatattributabletothenonsharedenvironment.
(Mostestimationproceduresconfoundthelattersource–nonsharedenvironmentalvariance,oftencallede2–withmeasurementerror,althoughtherearestatisticalwaysaroundthisproblem;seeRodgers,Rowe,&Li,1994.
)Twocriticalfeaturesofthisdenitionmustbeappreciated.
First,h2,c2,ande2dependonboththeamountofvariancerelevanttotheirconceptualdomain,andtheyalsodependontheamountofoverallvariance.
Theconceptualformulasareh2=geneticvariance/totalvariance,c2=sharedenvironmentalvariance/totalvariance,ande2=nonsharedvari-ance/totalvariance.
(Totalvarianceisoftenreferredtoas"phenotypicvariance"inthebehaviorgeneticliterature.
)Thesethreecoefcientsnecessarilyaddupto100percent,sothateachonemaybeinterpretedastheproportionoftotalvarianceattributabletogenetic,sharedenvironment,andnonsharedenvironmentsources,respectively.
Thus,forexample,ifgeneticvarianceremainsxed,butoverallvarianceinthedependentvariable(phenotypicvariance)increasessubstantially,thenh2goesdownduetothedenominator,withoutanyshiftinthegeneticcontributionitself.
Second,h2,c2,ande2mustbeinter-pretedinavariancecontext;thatis,thegeneticandenvironmentalcontributionsexplainindividualdifferences,andnotgeneralpropertiesoftraitsorbehaviors.
Forexample,genesperfectlydetermineeyecolor.
Yet,inasettingwithfewnon-brown-eyedpeople,heri-tabilitywouldbeverylow(becausethereislittlephenotypicvariation).
Inotherwords,h2indicateshowmuchvarianceinatraitorbehaviorisrelatedtogeneticsources.
Itdoesnotindicatewhethertheexistenceofthetraitorbehaviorhasanygeneticetiology.
Sim-ilarly,c2ande2indicatehowmuchvarianceinatraitorbehaviorisattributabletothesetwoenvironmentalsources,notwhetherthetraitorbehaviorhasunderlyingenviron-mentetiology.
Forexample,aperson'saccentisobviouslyinuencedbytheenvironment.
Butameasureofaccentwouldshowverylowc2,forthesimplereasonthatthereisverylittleoverallvariancetoexplain.
Evenotherwiseresponsiblebehaviorgeneticistsoftenmisusetheirlanguage,andreferto"geneticorenvironmentalinuencesonatrait,"when4JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardwhattheyreallymeanis"geneticorenvironmentalinuencesonvarianceinorindi-vidualdifferencesinatrait.
"Theexplanationabovereferstothebasicadditivegeneticmodel.
Thismodelimpliesthatanumberofseparategeneticsourceseachcontributeseparateandadditivepiecesofinuencesonthevarianceinatraitorbehaviorofinterest.
Ofcourse,somegeneticprocessesarenonlinearandnonadditive(e.
g.
,basicprinciplesofMendelianinheritanceareinherentlynon-additive).
Quantitativemodelinginbehaviorgeneticssupportsttingdominancemodels(e.
g.
,Neale&Cardon,1992),andtheconceptofemergenesishasbeenproposedtoaccountforconguralgeneticcontributions(Lykkenetal.
,1992).
Ontheotherhand,anothertypeofnonlineargeneticprocess,epistasis,involvesgeneticinter-actionsofallelesacrossgeneticloci(asopposedtoMendeliandominance,whichinvolvesinteractionsofalleleswithinageneticlocus).
Epistasisisdifculttomodelinbehavioralgeneticsettings(e.
g.
,Neale&Cardon,1992).
Inthisreview,wewillfocusoneffortstotadditivegeneticmodelstoadolescentdata,primarilybecauselittleattentionhasbeengiventononlinearornon-additivemodelsintheliterature.
Besidesgeneticnonadditiv-ity,asecondproblemoccurswhentherearegenetic-environmentalinteractionsthatarenotaccountedforwithinthemodel.
Turkheimer(1998)showedthattheeffectoffailingtoaccountforgenetic/environmentalinteractionsistobiasestimatesofh2andc2.
Whilethereislittlepublishedliterature,effortstoaccountforgene-environmentinteractionsareongoing(andseveralwillbementionedinreviewingtheliterature).
Bothh2andc2(and,implicitly,e2)canshiftandchangeovertimeandoverage,asthevariancesinboththenumeratorsandthedenominatorsshiftandchange.
Atadolescenceinparticular,phenotypicvarianceinmanytraitsandbehaviorscanshiftsubstantially.
Forafewyears,height(andmostotherphysiologicalmeasures)becomesmorehighlyvari-able,aschildrenreachpubertyatawiderangeofages.
Inlessobviousways,therecanbeshiftsinoverallvarianceofintelligence,manypersonalitytraits,andespeciallyinsocialandhealthbehaviors.
Smokingprovidesagoodexample.
Among8-year-olds,smokingbehaviorhasvirtuallynoheritability.
Obviously,thisisnotbecausegeneticinuencesdonotcontributetosmokingingeneral,butbecausethereislittlephenotypicvarianceinmeasuresofsmokingamong8-year-olds.
Byage14,smokingheritabilitiesbegintobedetectableinameasurelikeage-at-rstcigarette(butnotsomuchinmeasuresofsmokingaddiction).
Byage20,measuresofsmokingintensity,smokingduration,andsmokingaddictionallshowheritablecomponents.
Thesepointsaboutgeneticinuencesapplyequallytobothsharedandnonsharedenvironmentalinuencesaswell.
Itisinthiscontextthatwerefertothe"shiftingsand"onwhichweobservegeneticandenvironmentalinuences.
Thereareseveralproperwaystotreattheseproblems.
Therstisinterpretational;authorsmustbeclearaboutwhath2,c2,ande2mean,andwhattheydon'tmean.
Second,therearedirectmeasuresoftheseinuences;forexample,somebehaviorgeneticistsemphasizetheimportanceofcomputingheritabilities,andalsocomputingcoefcientsofgeneticvariation,whicharenotaffectedbytheoverallvarianceoftheDVitself(see,e.
g.
,Houle,1992).
Third,theshiftsinh2andc2valuescanprovideagreatdealofinforma-tioninandofthemselves.
Forexample,Kohler,Rodgers,andChristensen(1999)docu-mentedasubstantialshiftinh2offertility,usingseculartwindatafromDenmark.
Thedramaticupwardshiftsinh2valuesthattheyobservedoccurredsimultaneouslywiththeBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment5twofertilitytransitionsinDenmark,whichprovidedaframeworkwithinwhichtoexplainthisrapidchangeinheritability.
Thedifcultiesdenedinthisintroductorysectionareallrelatedtothemethod(and,importantly,toimproperinterpretationofthedesignandpurposeofthemethod).
The"shiftingsand"problemofinterpretingheritabilitiesisonlyaproblemifitisallowedtobe.
Wewillattempttocarefullyextracttheproperinformationfromthestudieswereview,andweinvitethereadertobringhealthycriticalinspectionintothisinvestigationwithus.
AsMaccoby(2000)noted,"knowingonlythestrengthofgeneticfactors.
.
.
isnotasufcientbasisforestimatingenvironmentalones"(p.
1).
Turkheimer(1998)suggestedthatheritabilitystudiesleadonlytothe"banaltautologythatallbehaviorisultimatelybasedinthegenotypeandbrain"(p.
782).
WhilewearemoresanguineaboutthevalueofsuchstudiesthanTurkheimer,weapplaudtheskepticalapproachthatthispositionimplies.
Finally,weintendtorespondtothe"shiftingsand"problembyrmlytyingdownourtreatmentwiththeoreticalorientationsfrombothsocialandbiologicaldomainsthatfocusonthetopicandnotonthemethod.
Thestudyofadolescenceingeneral,andespeciallyadolescentdevelopment,focusesattentionontheprocessofchange.
Wehaveusedthreedifferentsocialtheoriesinourpastworktoframetheprocessofadolescentdevelopment.
Moreproperly,theseareclosertomotivatingorientationsthantheyaretoformaltheo-ries,becauseweinnosensetestorevaluatethestructures.
Rather,weletthemguideourinvestigationandreview.
ThethreeapproachesaretheTransitionBehaviorPerspective,theLifeCoursePerspective,andProblemBehaviorTheory.
Inaddition,thebiologicalperspectivethatbestinformsthestudyofadolescentdevelopmentistoconsiderhormonalinuencesthataffectbothphysiologicalchangesandbehavioralchanges.
TheoreticalFrameworkAdolescenceembeddedinasocialandtemporalecologyTheTransitionBehaviorPerspectiveisdevelopedmorecompletelyinEnsminger(1987)andinRodgersandRowe(1993).
Thisorientingframeworkviewsadolescenceasaperiodofexpandedbehavioralopportunity.
Adolescentsbegintohavechoiceswithinabehav-ioralecologythatwerenotavailabletothemduringchildhood.
Howmuchtostudy,whethertosmokeanddrink,whatpartiestoattend,whatschoolclubstojoin,andman-agementofhealthbehaviorsareemergingissueswithinthedecision-makingframeworkofmanyadolescents.
Inaddition,consumerbehavioroffersexpandedopportunityinado-lescenceaswell.
Transitionbehaviorsaredenedasbehaviorsthatadolescentsuse–eitherovertlyorimplicitly–tosignalimpendingadulthood.
Thesebehaviorsmayhaveotherpurposesaswell,butatleastpartoftheirstatusistosociallyrepresentthetransitionalfeaturesofadolescence.
Someofthosebehaviorsareunhealthy,oratleastsociallypro-scribed.
Examplesincluderiskysexualbehavior,recklessdriving,smoking,drinkingtoexcess,druguse,andcheatingonhomework.
Sometransitionbehaviorsarehealthyandsociallynormative.
Examplesincludeplayingintheschoolband,checkingoutlibrary6JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardbooks,playingonatennisteam,andjoiningachurchgroup.
ThereasontheTransitionBehaviorPerspectiveisvaluableinthecontextofthecurrentreviewisthatisorientsattentiontowardbehaviorsthatmanyconsidertobeprototypicaladolescentbehavior.
Mostoftheattentiontotransitionbehaviorsinthesocialscience(andbehaviorgenetic)literatureisonbehaviorsthatwehavepreviouslylabeledmildlyandseverelydeviant(seeRodgers,Billy,&Udry,1984;Rodgers&Rowe,1993).
Whilethesociallyappropriatetransitionbehaviorsdeservemuchmoreattentionthantheyhavereceived,wewillfocusonthosethathavereceivedsubstantialresearchattention.
Thoseincludesmoking,drinking,druguse,anddating/sexualbehavior.
Asecondtheoreticalperspectivehelpsmotivatesomeofthebehaviorgeneticliteratureinthisdomain.
ProblemBehaviorTheory(Jessor&Jessor,1977)suggeststhatanumberofbothmildlyandseverelydeviantbehaviorsmaygrouptogether.
ThequestionofwhetherproblembehaviorsgrouptogetherwasexpandedbyMoft(1993)intoatheoryof"adolescence-limited"versus"life-course-persistent"deviantbehavior.
RodgersandRowe(1990)foundthatsexualbehaviorhadasomewhatunusualstatusinthecontextofProblemBehaviorTheory;whileitdidcovarywithothermildlydeviantbehaviors(e.
g.
,smoking,drinking,anddrivingillegally),italsocontainedsubstantialuniquevarianceofitsownthatdidnotoverlapwiththoseothervariables.
Themethodologyofbehaviorgeneticsprovidesapowerfulapproachtoinvestigatingthisperspective,andwewillreviewseveralstudiesthatconsiderthemultivariaterelationshipsbetweentwoormoreproblembehaviors.
AthirdtheoreticalapproachthatisusefulistheLifeCoursePerspective,whichsuggeststhatnormsareestablishedbysocietytodeneage-appropriatebehavioraltransitions;"agedifferentiationisexpressedinthesequenceofrolesandevents,socialtransitions,andturningpointsthatdepictthelifecourse"(Elder,1975).
HoganandAstone(1986)suggestedthattransitionsfromadolescenceintoandthroughadulthoodhaveorderedstages,andthatsocietyhasnormativeexpectationsaboutage-appropriatelifetransitions;Rindfuss,Swicegood,andRosenfeld(1987)investigateddeviationsfromthosenormativeorderedstages.
MostoftheexamplesofeventsofhighsalienceintheLifeCoursePerspectivearedemographictransitionssuchasmarriage,rstchild,oredu-cation.
IncombiningtheTransitionBehaviorandtheLifeCoursePerspective,webringawholenewdomainofbehaviorsunderthisumbrella.
Inthiscombinedperspective,therstcigarette,therstdrink,andlossofvirginitycanalsobeconsideredtohave"lifecoursestatus,"inthatweorganizeourthinkingandplansinrelationtothetimingoftheseevents.
Further,theyareofparticularsalienceindeningthe"adolescentexperi-ence,"anobservationthatbringsProblemBehaviorTheoryintothisintegrativeframe-work.
Indeed,wesuspectthatmanyindividuals(bothadultsandadolescentsthemselves)woulddenethesemildlyandseverelydeviantbehaviorsascloserto"adolescentproto-typebehaviors"thanthemoresociallynormativetransitionbehaviorslikejoiningclubsandplayingintheband.
Nevertheless,wenotetheexistenceofcertaincognitiveandpersonalitytransitionsthatmayalsobemotivatedbyTransitionBehaviorandLifeCourseperspectives.
CertainsubjectsinschoolmaybeperceivedinaLifeCourseperspective.
Oneexamplewouldbebrightstudentstakingpre-calculusin11thgrade,orAdvancedPlacementEnglishasseniors.
Anotherwouldbethetransitioninjunior-ormid-hightohavingastudyhallBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment7(andassociatedexpectations)aspartoftheschoolexperience.
Athirdwouldbeexpand-ingsocialautonomy,suchasthechoicetoattendaschooldanceortohavelunchoff-campus.
Afourthwouldbetheemergenceofleadershipinahigh-schoolsocial/politicalstructure.
AdolescenceembeddedinahormonalecologyTheideaofadolescent"raginghormones"hasbeenoverplayed;forexample,mostado-lescentsarenotnearlyassexuallyactiveinresponsetothosehormones(eventhosewhoarenonvirginsarenotnearlyassexuallyactive)asmanymediaportrayalsmightsuggest(see,e.
g.
,Rodgers,1996).
Theproperphraseisprobablycloserto"changinghormones.
"Indeed,hormonesdrivemanyofthephysiologicalchangesduringadolescence,whichinturnhavetremendousimpactsonbehavioralprocesses.
Buchanan,Eccles,andBecker(1992)developedanintegratedframeworkinwhichtoviewtheinuencethathormonalchangesduringadolescencehaveonadolescentbehav-iorsandtraits.
TheynotedthatHistorically,mostofthechangesinmoodandbehaviorwerepresumedtobenegativeandtobetheresultofbiologicalfactors,particularlyofhormones.
.
.
.
Morerecently,psychol-ogistshavequestionedboththeprevalenceofsuchnegativechangesandtheirhypothesizedbiologicalroots.
.
.
.
Emphasishasshiftedtocontextual(i.
e.
,family,school,peergroup)andpsychological(i.
e.
,self-esteem,genderroleorientation)factors.
(p.
62)Theirreviewshowshormonalchangestopotentiallyinuenceadolescentself-esteem,happiness,concentration,aggressionandbehaviorproblems,andsocialrelationships.
Udryetal.
(1985)showedalinkbetweenandrogenichormonesandmaleadolescentsexualbehavior,andUdryandTalbert(1988)documentedpersonalityresponsestohormonalchanges.
Susmanetal.
(1985)showedalinkbetweenadolescenthormonelevelsandsocio-emotionalbehaviors.
IntroductorysummaryBehaviorgeneticmodelingtoooftenoccursinitsownvacuum.
Thisintroductionwasdesignedtocreatealargercontextfortheupcomingreviewofbehaviorgeneticstudiesofadolescentbehaviorsandtraits.
Genesinuencehormones.
Hormoneshavebothover-lappingvariancewithandcanchangetraitsandbehaviors.
Alloccuratanorganiclevel,withinanindividualwhoisembeddedinasocialenvironmentoffamily,friends,school,church,andothersocialinuences.
Further,allofthesecomplexinterrelationsaredenedtemporally,andmaymeandifferentthingsatdifferentagesandstages.
TheLifeCourseandTransitionBehaviorPerspectives,alongwithProblemBehaviorTheory,canhelpusappreciatethiscomplexinterplay.
Recentworkinthebehaviorgeneticliteraturehasshiftedourorientationawayfromastrictcausalowfromthegenometobehavior,andhassubstitutedvariouscomplexandfascinatingfeedbackloopsinvolvedinthecausal8JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardprocess(see,e.
g.
,Brown,1999;Gottlieb,2000).
Wewillreturntothisbroaderperspec-tiveinaconcludingsection,whereweplaceincontexttheresultsofourreviewofgeneticandenvironmentalinuencesthatemergefromthebehaviorgeneticliterature.
MethodologyUsedinThisReviewToidentifyrelevantempiricalstudiestobereviewedinthischapter,wepartitionedourtitleintothreecomponentkeywords:"behaviorgenetic,""adolescence,"and"develop-ment.
"Ourinitialliteraturereviewidentiedprimaryjournalsineacharea:BehaviorGeneticsfortherst,Adolescenceforthesecond,andDevelopmentalPsychologyforthethird.
Weobtainedcopiesofeacharticlethatinvolvedbehaviorgeneticanalysisofatopicrelatedtoadolescentdevelopmentfrom1985to2000.
Inreviewingthosearticles,weidentiedanumberofadditionalarticlesthatwereobtainedaswell.
Inaddition,severalrecentchaptersinAnnualReviewofPsychologyprovidedbothmaterialandreferencestosupportthisreview(seeMaccoby,2000;Plomin&Rende,1991;Rose,1995;Steinberg&Morris,2001).
ByfarthemajorityofourarticlesreviewedherecomefromBehaviorGeneticsandDevelopmentalPsychology.
Further,mostarerecentarticles.
Thoughwesearchedforrele-vantarticlesineachareabackto1985,mostthatwererelevanthadbeenpublishedmorerecentlythanthat,manyinthelatterhalfofthe1990s.
BehaviorGeneticResearchonAdolescence:TheSocial/BehavioralEcologyThesocial/behavioraldomainsrepresentedwithinthearticleswehavecollectedincludeadolescenttransitionbehaviors(smoking,drinking,sexualbehavior,andotherrisk-takingactivities),social/mentalhealthamongadolescents(includingdepressionandantisocialbehavior),indicatorsofsocialrelationshipswithinthefamily(parentsandsiblings),andtreatmentofcognitiveandpersonalitydevelopmentduringadolescence.
TransitionbehaviorsBehaviorgeneticstudieshaveshownthatthereisgeneticvarianceunderlyingadolescenttransitionbehaviors,andhavealsoindicatedsomeofthedynamicsunderlyingthesocial/environmentalcorrelates.
Whileageneralconsiderationofproblembehaviorsspreadsoutbeyondtheboundariescoveredbytransitionbehavior,theycertainlyoverlapsubstantiallywiththatdomain,especiallythroughalcoholanddruguse.
Withinthedomainofproblembehaviors,Gjoneetal.
(1996)usedtheChildBehav-iorChecklist(CBCL)tomeasureinternalizingandexternalizingproblembehaviorsamongNorwegiansame-sextwinsbornduringthe1970sand1980s.
TheirresultsshowedBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment9signicantheritabilitiesforbothdomains,withincreasingh2anddecreasingc2astheseverityoftheproblembehaviorincreasedforbothinternalizingandexternalizingbehav-iors(althoughrescalingofthosevariablesdampenedthispattern).
VandenOord,Boomsma,andVerhulst(1994)alsousedtheCBCLwithasampleofinternationaladoptedchildrenintheNetherlands.
LikeGjoneetal.
,theyfoundsignicantheritabil-ityforexternalizingbehaviors;however,theydidnotmatchtheirresultforinternalizingbehaviors.
Theyalsofoundlargervarianceformalesthanforfemalesforaggressivebehav-ior,whichtheyspeculatedmighthavegeneticorigins.
Eavesetal.
(1993)studiedconductdisorderitemsfromtheRutterParentQuestionnaireusing8–16-year-oldmaletwinsfromtheVirginiaAdolescentBehavioralDevelopmentstudy.
Theirmodelidentiedfourunderlyinglatentclasses,whichwerethemselvesshowntobeheritable.
However,theirmodelrejectedaunidimensionalinterpretationofthelatentprocessesunderlyingconductdisorder.
Rodgers,Rowe,andLi(1994)studiedmeasuresobtainedfromtheBehavioralProblemIndex(BPI)using5–11-year-oldchildrenfromrespondentsintheNationalLon-gitudinalSurveyofYouth(NLSY),nationallyrepresentativedatafromadolescentsbornbetween1958and1965.
ForthesixsubscalesoftheBPI,theyfoundstrongheritabili-tiesforthemoretrait-basedsubscales(Anxiety,Hyperactivity,andDependent)andweakerheritabilitiesforthesocialsubscales(Antisocial,PeerConict,andHeadstrong).
Further,thethreesocialsubscalesshowedsignicantsharedenvironmentalvariance.
Thequalityofthehomeenvironment(measuredbytheHOME)showedsignicantnon-sharedinuencesonsiblingsinthesamehouseholdforallofthesubscalesexceptforPeerConict.
VanderValketal.
(1998)usedanadoptiondesignwith10–18-year-oldDutchchildrentoinvestigatethelongitudinalstabilityofproblembehaviors.
TheirdependentmeasurescamefromtheChildBehaviorChecklist.
Theyfoundstabilityinanexternal-izingfactor,primarilygeneticinorigin,andmoresharedenvironmentalinuenceattheearlyagesthanatthelaterones.
Rowe,Almeida,andJacobson(1999)studiedadolescentaggressionusingtheAddHealthdata.
Theyfoundanh2=.
32,withlittlesharedenvir-onmentalvariance.
Theheritabilityincreasedfromthisvaluewithincreasesinfamilywarmthmeasuredattheschoollevel.
Finally,vandenOordandRowe(1997)investi-gatedsocialmaladjustmentusingtheBPImeasuresintheNLSY.
Theirndingssuggestedthatnonsharedenvironmentalinuenceshadthemosteffectonchildren'sproblembehav-iors.
Theyalsofoundsupportfora"liabilitymodel,"suggesting"astableunderlyingliabilitymaybethe'thirdvariable'thatexplainstherelationsbetweensubsequentlevelsofproblembehaviors"(pp.
319–320).
Severalstudiesaddressedspecictransitionbehaviors.
Koopmansetal.
(1999)studiedsmokinginitiationandquantityusingatwinstudyofDutchadolescents.
Theytabivari-atemodel,andfoundthattherewereseparatedimensionsunderlyingsmokinginitiationandsmokingquantity.
Theyfoundsubstantialh2=.
39andevengreaterc2=.
54forsmokinginitiation,whileh2andc2becamenegligibleforsmokingquantity.
Thisnding–thatthereareseparate(thoughpotentiallyoverlapping)liabilitiesforsmokingonsetandsmokingpersistence–wasalsoobtainedbyMaddenetal.
(1999)andHeathetal.
(1999)usingadultsamples.
Vikenetal.
(1999)usedtwinsfromaFinnishbirthcohortbornin1975–9tostudyself-reportedalcoholconsumption.
Likethesmokingresultsabove,theyfoundthatsharedenvironmentalinuenceswereimportantindrinkinginitiation,butthatadditivegenetic10JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardeffectsbecamemoreimportantinexplainingvarianceindrinkingfrequencyamongthosewhohadalreadybegundrinking.
BusterandRodgers(2000)usedmeasuresoflightandheavydrinkingfromadolescentsintheNLSY.
Theyfoundsignicanth2foradolescentmalesintheNLSYforlightdrinking,withtheshifttosignicantc2forheavydrinking.
Adolescentfemaleshadastronggeneticbasis(andnon-signicantc2's)forbothlightandheavydrinking.
Carey(1992)andMeyerandNeale(1992)usedasimulateddatasettoinvestigateonsetofdruguseamongteenagers.
Careysimulatedthedatasettorepresentthreeprocesses,diffusion/exposure,initialuse,andpersistence.
MeyerandNealetmodelstoshowthat,atleastinthisarticialdataset,thesharedenvironmentaccountedfortwinsimilarityindruguseonsetandtiming.
Whilemultiplestagetheoriesthatincludesocialinuenceprocesseshavebeendevel-opedtoexplainthespreadofadolescenttransitionbehaviorslikesmokinganddrinking(e.
g.
,Rowe&Rodgers,1991;Roweetal.
,1996),thebasicandsimpledistinctionbetweenonsetandmaintenanceappearstobethemostabidingandvaluableonetoemergefromthisinvestigation(e.
g.
,Mayhew,Flay,&Mott,2000).
Thecoherentresultacrossthesestudiesisthatonsetisdrivensocially,butafterinitiationhasoccurred,thevarianceintheamountisgeneticallybased.
Asdiscussedabove,notalltransitionbehaviorsarenecessarilyproblembehaviors.
Participationinsportsisapositivetransitionbehavior(alongwithotherextracurricularactivities,churchprograms,hobbies,etc.
).
Boomsmaetal.
(1989)studiedparticipationinsportsusingteenagedtwinsfromAmsterdamandtheirparents.
Theyfoundageneticcomponenttosportsparticipation,andastrongsharedenvironmentalcomponentforfemales(butnotformales).
Further,theydevelopedabivariatemodelbetweensportsparticipationandheartrate,andfoundheartratetohaveastrongergeneticbasisandsportsparticipationtohaveastrongerenvironmentalbasis.
Weencourageadditionalresearchfallingintothisdomain,inwhichhealthyadolescenttransitionbehaviorsareevaluatedthroughbehaviorgeneticandothermethodologies.
Sexualbehaviorisatransitionbehaviorthatbecomessociallynormativewithincreas-ingage.
Severalbehaviorgeneticstudieshaveidentiedsignicantheritabilitiesformea-suresofage-at-rst-intercourse(e.
g.
,Dunneetal.
,1997;Rodgers,Rowe,&Buster,1999),whichtypicallyoccursduringadolescenceintheculturesstudied.
Milleretal.
(1999)showedarelationshipbetweendopaminereceptorsandageatrstintercourse,provid-inginformationaboutapotentialgeneticmechanismtohelpexplainthislink.
Social/mentalhealthRendeetal.
(1993)tbiometricalmodelstomeasuresofdepressioninadolescents.
Theyfoundasignicantheritabilityintheoveralldepressiondistribution.
However,thegeneticcomponentdisappearedwhenmodelswerettoevaluateextremedepression.
Inotherwords,theredidnotseemtobeanydifferentoradditionalgeneticcomponenttoextremedepressionoverandaboveitsstatusasanextremeformoftheoverallvarianceindepres-sion.
Pikeetal.
(1996)usedUSsiblingpairswhowerewithinfouryearsofoneanotherinage,includingtwins,siblings,half-siblings,andunrelatedsiblings.
DepressionwasBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment11measuredinthreeways,usingtheChildDepressionInventory,theBPI-Depressionsub-scale,andtheBehaviorEventsInventory(BEI).
Theyfoundthatthe"modelattributesthevarianceofdepressivesymptomstosubstantialgeneticinuence,negligiblesharedenvironmentalinuence,andmoderatenonsharedenvironmentalinuence"(p.
597).
Further,theyalsofoundthatmother'snegativitywasassociatedwithadolescentdepres-sionthroughthenonsharedenvironment,independentofgenesandthesharedenviron-ment.
Resultsforfather'snegativityweresimilar.
JacobsonandRowe(1999)usedthekinshipstructureintheNationalLongitudinalSurveyofAdolescentHealth(AddHealth)tostudytherelationbetweensocialconnectednessandadolescentdepression.
Theyfounddifferentmodelsformalesandfemales.
Geneticinuenceswerestrongerforfemalesthanformalesforbothdepressionindicators,andalsoforthecovariationbetweensocialconnectednessanddepression.
Other(overlapping)literaturehasinvestigatedantisocialbehaviorinadolescents.
ThestudybyPikeetal.
(1996)reviewedabovethattreateddepressionalsoinvestigatedanti-socialbehaviorusingsubscalesfromtheBPIandtheBEI.
Antisocialbehaviorshowedmoresharedenvironmentalvariancethandiddepression,butgeneticandnonsharedenvi-ronmentalinuencesalsoaccountedforsignicantvariance.
Aswithdepression,thenon-sharedenvironmentaccountedforcovariationbetweenmother'snegativityandantisocialbehavior,andgeneticandsharedenvironmentalinuencescontributedvarianceaswell.
InastudybyO'Connoretal.
(1998)usingColoradoAdoptionProject(CAP)datafromlatechildhoodandearlyadolescence(ages7–12),antisocialbehaviorwasassessedusingconstructedmeasuresforparentalantisocialbehaviorandtheChildBehaviorChecklist(CBC)forchildren.
Theywereinterestedinthecausaldirectionalityofparentingbehav-iorandchildren'santisocialbehavior.
Theyfoundthatthecovariationbetweennegativeparentingandantisocialbehaviorwasnotevokedbythechild;however,theirresultswereconsistentwithaplausibleparentaleffectonchildren'santisocialbehavior.
Finally,astudybyTopolskietal.
(1997)useddatafromtheVirginiaTwinStudyofAdolescentBehavioralDevelopmenttostudyseparationanxietydisorder(SAD),over-anxiousdisorder,andmanifestanxiety.
Moderateheritabilitieswerefoundforeachofthethree,withmeaningfulsharedenvironmentalvarianceforSAD.
Therewerenostrongageorgenderdifferences.
SocialrelationshipswithfamilymembersTherehasbeensubstantialresearchonadolescentsfromabehaviorgeneticperspectiveonfamilyrelationships.
BoththeTransitionBehaviorandLifeCourseperspectivesprovidemotivationforchangingrelationshipstoemergebetweenadolescentsandtheirfamily.
Becauseadolescentshaveanewandbroaderbehavioralrepertoire,becausetheyarebeginningthetransitionintotheindependenceandautonomyofadulthood,andbecausesocietynotesseveralspecicmarkersduringadolescenceasparticularlysalient(e.
g.
,transitiontohighschool,rstcar,leavinghome,etc.
),thesesocialrelationshipsmaybesubtle,volatile,and/ordynamic.
Inaninterestingmethodologicalstudy,Plominetal.
(1994)showedthatanumberofmeasuresofthefamilyenvironmentinfactcontainsubstantialgeneticvariation:"On12JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardaverage,morethanaquarterofthevarianceoftheseenvironmentalmeasurescanbeaccountedforbygeneticdifferencesamongchildren"(p.
32).
Thisstudy,apartoftheNonsharedEnvironmentinAdolescentDevelopment(NEAD)project,recruitedadoles-centsfrombothnon-divorcedandstepfamiliesrandomlychosenfromtheUSpopula-tion.
Thisprojectgrewoutofalargeeffortshowingtheimportanceofnonsharedenvironmentalinuencesonindividualdifferencesinhuman(includingadolescent)behavior.
Thisparticularstudydemonstratedthedifcultyincleanlyseparatingmeasuresintogeneticandenvironmentalcategories.
Elkins,McGue,andIacono(1997)tookadevelopmentalperspectiveinastudyofparent–sonrelationshipsduringadolescence.
TheyusedtheMinnesotaTwinFamilyStudyandaParentalEnvironmentQuestionnairethatassessedvariousaspectsoftheparent–childrelationship.
Theyfounddifferentetiologiesfortwinsaroundage11com-paredtothosearoundage17.
Bothagesshowedheritabilityofadolescents'perceptionsofthequalityofparent–sonrelationships,withsubstantiallyhigherh2fortheoldertwins.
Theseeffectswerestrongerforthefather–sonrelationshipthanforthemother–sonrela-tionship.
Neiderhiseretal.
(1998)useddatafromsame-sexsiblingsintheNEADprojectdescribedabovetostudyadolescentperceptionsofparenting.
Adolescentperceptionsofparentingdidmediateparentconictmeasuresandadolescentantisocialmeasures,andtheassociationbetweenparentalandchildmaladjustmenthadastronggeneticcompo-nent.
Busselletal.
(1999)usedthesamedatasourcetoinvestigatethebasisforthecommonndingthatparent–childrelationshipsarerelatedtothequalityofsiblingrelationships.
Mostofthecovariancebetweenqualityofthemother–childrelationshipandthequalityofthesiblingrelationshipwasattributabletothesharedenvironment,althoughsignicantgeneticandnonsharedenvironmentalcomponentswereidentiedaswell.
Neale(1999)challengedsomeoftheassumptionsfromtheBusselletal.
study,butsupportedthendingoftheimportanceofthesharedenvironmentforbothsiblingandparent–childrelationships(seealsoNeiderhiseretal.
,1999,whorepliedtoNeale'scriticisms).
PersonalityAnumberofresearchershavetbiometricalmodelsbasedonbehaviorgeneticdesignstomeasuresofpersonality.
McGue,Bacon,andLykken(1993)useddatafromtwinsinMinnesotaHighSchoolsduringthe1970s,measuredinlateadolescenceandthenaroundtenyearslater.
TheyusedtheMultidimensionalPersonalityQuestionnaire,whichhassubscalesofpositiveemotionality(similartoextraversion),negativeemotionality(similartoneuroticism/aggression),andconstraint(harmavoidanceandtraditionalism).
Theyfoundreductioningeneticinuenceoverthetwoageperiodsfornegativeemotionality,stabilityinoverallpersonalitystructurethatwasprimarilybasedongeneticprocesses,andchangeinpersonalitystructurethatwasprimarilybasedonenvironmentalfactors.
Billigetal.
(1996)useddatafrom17-year-oldmaletwinsfromtheMinnesotaTwin/FamilyStudywithmeasuresofpersonality(obtainedfromtheMultidimensionalPersonalityQuestionnaire)andasecondsurveycalledLifeEventsInterviewforAdolescents.
Inthissecondinstrument,respondentsindicatedwhichofawidevarietyoflifeeventstheyhadBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment13experienced,whichwerethemselvesdividedintofamilyevents(e.
g.
,thewholefamilymovedintoanewhouse),nonfamilyeventsindependentoftherespondent'sbehavior(e.
g.
,aclosefriendmovedaway),andnonfamilyeventsnotindependentoftherespon-dent'sbehavior(e.
g.
,suspendedfromschool).
Biometricalmodelingshowedageneticbasistononindependentnonfamilylifeeventsandgeneticcovariancebetweennoninde-pendentnonfamilylifeeventsandpersonality(especiallywiththepersonalityfactor,constraint).
Finally,Macaskilletal.
(1994)usedtheEysenckPersonalityScaleswithAus-traliantwinsaged11to18.
Afterpartiallingoutageandgender,theyfoundgeneticinu-enceforpsychoticismandneuroticism.
Othertreatmentofmorespecicpersonalitytopicscanalsobefound.
Koopmansetal.
(1995)studiedsensationseekingusingDutchtwinsaged12–24andtheirparents.
TheyusedZuckerman'sSensationSeekingScale,whichhasseveralnon-overlappingsub-scales.
Theyfoundthat"genesplayamajorroleintheindividualdifferencesinsensationseeking"(p.
354),replicatingresultsfromFulker,Eysenck,andZuckerman.
(1980).
Nosharedenvironmentalinuencesweresignicant.
Eavesetal.
(1997)useda28-itemsocialattitudesurveytostudyconservatism.
Theyfoundanimportantagedifference,withtwinsyoungerthan20havingtheirconservatismrelatedtosharedenvironmentalfactors,whilethoseolderthan20hadconservatismvariancethatwasprimarilyrelatedtogeneticinuence.
CognitiveabilitiesUsinganEgyptiansampleoftwinsaged12–19,Abdel-Rahim,Nagoshi,andVandenberg(1990)studiedmeasuresfromabroadbatteryofcognitivemeasures.
Theyfounddiffer-entresultsfromthoseobtainedfromWesternstudies,withlittledifferencebetweenMZandDZtwinscoresandlowerMZcorrelationsingeneral.
Theyprovideacross-culturalinterpretationofthisresult,althoughtheyhastentonoteseveralmethodologicalconcerns,includinglowsamplesizeandabsenceofheightheritability.
NagoshiandJohnson(1993)usedfamilydatafromtheHawaiiFamilyStudyofCognitionalongwith(age-corrected)measuresofverbalability,spatialability,perceptualspeed,andvisualmemory.
Theyfoundaracedifference(betweenthoseofCaucasianancestryandthoseofJapaneseancestry),withsimilarc2foradolescentsandadults.
PetrilandThompson(1993)usedtwindatafromtheWesternReserveTwinProjectcognitivemeasuresfromtheWISC-R,theColoradoTestofSpecicCognitiveAbilities,theMetropolitanAchievementTest,andtheCognitiveAbilitiesTest.
Univariateanalysisshowedbothgeneticandsharedenvi-ronmentalvarianceunderlyingindividualdifferencesincognitionandachievement,andmultivariateanalysisshowingcovariancebetweenthem(especiallygeneticinuence).
Rodgers,Rowe,andMay(1994)usedPPVT,PIAT,andDigitSpanmeasuresfromchil-drenandthosetransitioningintoadolescents(aged5–12)intheNLSY-Childrendatasettostudyintelligence/achievement.
Theyfoundmoderateh2(medianh2=.
50)andsmallerc2(medianc2=.
16)acrossveabilitymeasures.
Theirparticularfocus,however,wasonthenonsharedenvironmentalinuences.
Theyusedspecicmeasuresofthenonsharedenvironment,includingdifferencesamongsiblingsintripstothemuseum,owningbooks,parentalreading,spanking,andHOMEscores.
Theyfoundasignicantrelationshipof14JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
BardbookstothePIATReadingRecognitionsubscale,andasignicantrelationshipoftripstothemuseumtothePIATMathsubscale.
Plominetal.
(1997)usedalongitudinalsampleofColoradoadoptiveandbiologicalchildrentostudybiometricalstabilityacrossages1–16.
Theyfoundthatovertime,chil-drenbecamemoreliketheirparentsincognitiveperformance.
Further,duringadoles-cence,adoptivechildrenbecamesimilartotheirbiologicalparents,suggestingthat"genesthatstablyaffectcognitiveabilitiesinadulthooddonotallcomeintoplayuntiladoles-cence"(p.
442).
Inameta-analysisofliteraturefrom1967–85,McCartney,Harris,andBernieri(1990)foundthattheimportanceofsharedenvironmentasitcontributedtodifferencesinIQdecreasedwithage.
BehaviorGeneticResearchonAdolescence:TheBiological/HormonalEcologyVeryrecently,thehumangenomehasbeenmapped.
Thiseffortstimulatedbothknowl-edgeofandinterestinthewayourhumangeneticstructureinuenceshumanbehavior.
Criticshavelongdecriedeffortstolinkgeneticstructuretobehavioraloutcomes.
Moreproperly,weshouldsimplyunderstandthatgeneticinuenceswillshowupinvirtuallyalldomains.
AsTurkheimer(1998)notes,"Everythingisbiological;everythingisgenetic"(p.
789).
Hedidnotmean,ofcourse,thateverythingisonlygenetic.
Knowledgeofthehumangenomepermitsspecicationofmechanisms.
Specicgeneticlocihavebeenidentiedashavinginuence(inacorrelationalratherthandeter-ministicsense)onanumberofadolescentbehaviorsthathavebeentreatedinthisreview,includingrisk-taking,smoking,andalcoholuse.
Behaviorgeneticsoffersalessdirectindi-catorofgeneticinvolvementthanmoleculargeneticmethods.
Ontheotherhand,theQTL(QuantitativeTraitLoci)methodsimplycorrelatesstructureinthegenomewithmeasuredtraitsofinterest.
Anumberof"falseleads"haveemergedusingQTLstudies,althoughthemethodwillcertainlybevaluableinthelongrun.
Activityinbothmolec-ularandbehavioralgeneticarenashasacceleratedduringthepastdecade,andinmanywaysthetwoapproachescomplementoneanother.
PhysicalgrowthAnumberofbehaviorgeneticstudieshavebeenmadeofadolescentdevelopmentfromabiologicalperspective.
Mostofthesestudieshavetreatedmeasuresofgrowthorotherbiologicalmarkersthatwouldbeexpectedtohavestronggeneticinuences.
Inthesecases,theimportantquestionsareoftenwhetherthereareanyenvironmentalinuencesofnote.
Analysisofweightdevelopmentandweightgainisagoodcaseinpoint.
Whileadultweighthasastrongheritablecomponent(h2equalsaround.
80inonereview;seeGrilo&Pogue-Geile,1991),itseemsreasonablethatenvironmentalinuencemightalsoaffectweight,andmanyofthosemightreasonablyemergefromthefamily.
BehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment15JacobsonandRowe(1998)studiedadolescentBodyMassIndex(BMI)amongUSAddHealthrespondentsfromabehaviorgeneticstandpoint.
Theyfoundsubstantialher-itability,consistentwithpreviousstudies,andfoundevidencethatthegenesinuencingBMIaresimilarformalesandfemales.
However,theyfoundsomedifferencesinthegenetic/environmentapportionmentformales/femalesandforblacks/whites.
Further,theyfoundanimportantsourceofsharedenvironmentalinuenceforwhitefemales.
Beunenetal.
(1998)studiedsubcutaneousfatdistributionusingBelgiantwinpairs.
Theymeasuredstature,weight,BMI,andvesubcutaneousskinfoldindicators.
Theyfoundgeneticandnonsharedenvironmentalvariancetounderlieindividualdifferencesinbodyfat,butnosharedenvironmentalinuences.
Theirresultssuggestedthatalloftheskinfoldswereinuencedbythesamesetofgenes.
Anothergrowthprocessisthatrelatedtopuberty.
Studiesofpubertaldevelopmentusingbehaviorgeneticmethodshavebeenconducted,althoughthosewillbereviewedbelowinoursectiononhumanreproduction.
HormonesHarris,Vernon,andBoomsma(1998)studiedtestosteroneinDutchtwin-parentdata.
UnliketheBMIndings,theyfounddifferentgeneticinuencesformalesandfemalesforplasmatestosteroneconcentrations,withheritabilitiesofaroundh2=.
60formalesandh2=.
40forfemales.
Differentgeneticfactorsappearedtoemergeinadulthoodformales,whiletheywerethesameforfemales.
Otherresearchonhormonesthatisnotdirectlytiedtobehaviorgeneticmethodsisneverthelessrelevanttothistreatment.
Udryandhiscolleagues(Udryetal.
,1985;Udry,Talbert,&Morris,1986)showedlinksbetweenandrogensandadolescentsexualbehavioramongbothmalesandfemales.
Sexualbehaviorandhumanreproductionoccurattheboundarybetweensocialandbiologicalprocesses(or,moreproperly,weshouldprobablysaythattheysubstantiallycrosstheboundary).
Thebiologicalmarkersignalingreproductivepotentialispuberty.
DoughtyandRodgers(2000)tbiometricalmodelstomeasuresofageatmenarcheforUSfemaleadolescentsfromtheNLSY.
Theyfoundasignicantandsubstantialheri-tability,withtherestofthevarianceattributabletothenonsharedenvironment/mea-surementerror.
Theyalsofoundthatfatherabsencewasrelatedtoageatmenarche,andingoriginallydevelopedbyBelskyandhiscolleagues(e.
g.
,Belsky,2000)andgivensubstantialattentionintheevolutionarypsychologyliterature.
RodgersandBuster(1994)foundaseasonalcomponenttomenarche,withdisproportionatenumbersofNLSYfemalesreportingrstmenstruationinthesummer.
Further,theyestimatedalargeheritabilityofh2=.
62forseasonalmenarche,andnosharedenvironmentalcomponent.
SexualityandhumanreproductionWehavereviewedseveralstudiesofageatrstintercourseearlierinoursectionontran-sitionbehaviors.
Inaddition,RodgersandDoughty(2000)didabiometricalanalysisofNLSYadolescentfertilityexpectations,fertilityideals,andfertilityoutcomes.
Theyfound16JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
Bardasubstantialheritabilityofh2=.
60foridealfertilityreportedatages14–21in1979,althoughtheestimatewasmuchlowerwhenitwasreassessedtwoyearslater.
Moderateheritabilitieswerefoundforlateadolescents(ages17–24in1982)forfertilityexpecta-tions.
ThoughfewoftheNLSYrespondentshadhadchildrenby1982,enoughhaddonesotoestimateheritabilitiesforthiscohortof17–24-year-olds;theyfoundaremarkablyhighh2=.
73,withnosharedenvironmentalvariance.
Obviously,anumberofadoles-centsexualandreproductivebehaviorshavegeneticcomponents,andingthatmightappeartosometobeinconsistentwiththetenetsofFisher'sFundamentalTheoremofNaturalSelection(Fisher,1930).
Thisinconsistencyisonlyapparentandnotreal,however.
Rodgers,Rowe,andMiller(2000)providebroadempiricaltreatmentandRodgersetal.
(2001)discusstheroleofgeneticinuencesonhumantnessandresolvetheapparentinconsistencywithFisher'stheorem,frombothbehaviorgeneticandmoleculargeneticstandpoints.
SummaryStatementsandConclusionBythemselves,behavioralgeneticstudiescanappearsomewhatsterile.
Atoneextreme,suchstudiesoftenreportthe"usualmoderateheritability,"theabsenceofanymeaning-fulsharedenvironment(oratleasttheinterpretablesharedenvironment),muchnon-sharedenvironmentalandmeasurementerror,andlittlebeyond(seeTurkheimer,2000,foraformalizationofthissetofndings).
Butattheotherextreme,theycanidentifyprocessesunderlyinghumanbehavior,suggestinterestingandintricategenetic/environ-mentalinteractions,showinterpretablegenderandracedifferences,complementstudiesfromotherdomains(suchasdevelopmentalpsychology,moleculargenetics,etc.
),andstronglyinformourmodelsofhumanbehavior.
Oneofthestrongestvaluesofbehaviorgeneticmodelingisphilosophical;thisapproachhashelpedtobreakresearchersoutoftheextremeandunhealthytendencytowardsocialdeterminism.
AsPlominandRende(1991)havenoted,behaviorgeneticmodelscanbeaspowerfulforstudyingtheenvi-ronmentasforstudyinggeneticinuences;inthatsense,itisamisnomertocallthissetofmodelsandmethodsbehavior"genetics.
"Thisreviewhasidentiedanumberofcoherentpatternsacrossstudies,whichwillbesummarizedhere.
Geneticinuencesareubiquitousintheadolescentdevelopmentprocess.
Thatstatementisnotsurprisinginregardtoprimarilybiologicaldomainslikepubertaldevelopment,hormones,andphysicaldevelopment.
But,interestingly,theher-itabilitiesformanysocialandbehavioralprocessesaregenerallyofthesamemagnitudeasthoseforthemorebiologicaldomains.
ThisresultstronglysupportsthepositiontakeninRodgers,Rowe,andLi(1994)withregardtostudiesofgeneticorenvironmentalinu-ences:"Eachtypeofinuencecan[i.
e.
,should]becontrolledinthestudyoftheother"(p.
374).
Innosensewouldbehaviorgeneticndingsofgeneticinuenceobviatetheimportanceofdevelopingsocialmodelsofadolescentdevelopment.
Butifthosesocialmodelsdonotcontrolfororotherwiseaccountfortheautomaticsimilarityamongrelatedkincausedbysharedgenes,thenthevalidityofthosendingsisthreatenedataveryfundamentallevel.
BehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment17Thisreviewhasalsoidentiedafewsharedenvironmentalinuences,andmanynon-sharedenvironmentalinuences.
TurkheimerandWaldron(2000)evaluatedthestatusofnonsharedenvironmentalinuences,andexpressedpessimismthatwewillidentifyspecicinuencesthathavemuchimportance.
Further,Turkheimer(1999)andMolenaar,Boomsma,andDolan(1997)haveshowntheeffectsoffailingtoaccountforgenetic/environmentalinteractions,whichcanbiasthemanyestimatesofheritabilityandsharedenvironmentalvarianceinstandardbiometricalmodels.
Thesearchisstillonforthespeciccausesofthelargeportionofvariancethatbehaviorgeneticistscall"thenonsharedenvironment.
"Wehavealsospeciedanumberofgenderdifferences,andotherdemographicsub-groupsshowdifferencesingeneticpartitioningaswell.
Inrelationtosexualorrepro-ductivebehaviors,suchdifferencesarevirtuallyaxiomatic.
Inotherdomains,theycanhelpidentifyusefultreatmentapproaches(e.
g.
,inrelationtomentalhealth),usefulinter-ventions(e.
g.
,inrelationtoproblembehaviors),orusefulcomponentsofbehavioralmodelsinbasicresearch.
Weconcludewithsomecommentsaboutthethreetheoreticalperspectiveswehaveusedrepeatedlyinpastresearchtoinformandorganizeourthinking:TransitionBehav-iorTheory,theLifeCoursePerspective,andProblemBehaviorTheory.
BothTransitionBehaviorTheoryandtheLifeCoursePerspectivesuggestthattherearesocialmarkerstowhichadolescentsattendintheirdevelopmentalprocess.
Examplesincludestartinghighschool,therstcigarette,puberty,initiationofsexualbehavior,andbeginningtodrive.
Itseemsclearthatthereisgeneticvarianceunderlyingvirtuallyalloftheindividualdif-ferencesinthesevariousbehaviors.
Somebehaviorandmoleculargeneticresearchreviewedabovehasevenbeenabletoevaluatewhethertherearesharedgeneticinuencescommontothesedifferentdomains.
Infact,weconsiderthatbivariateandmultivariatemodelsshowingthegeneticandenvironmentaloverlapprovidesomeofthemostexcit-ingandvaluablemodelstoapplytofuturekinshipdatausingbehaviorgeneticdesigns.
Anumberofsuchmodelshavebeendevelopedpreviously,andhaveprovidedvaluableandexcitingndings.
Otherswillfollow.
Adolescenceprovidesafascinating"age-gradedlaboratory"forthestudyofdevelop-mentalprocesses.
Behaviorgeneticmethodshavebeenfruitfullyappliedwithinthislaboratory.
Thendingsfromthosestudies,andthewaythosendingsinteractwiththoseoutsidetheboundariesofbehaviorgenetics,haveandwillcontinuetoprovidestimulat-ingandvaluablescience.
KeyReadingsBuster,M.
A.
,&Rodgers,J.
L.
(2000).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesonalcoholuse:DFanalysisofNLSYkinshipdata.
JournalofBiosocialScience,32,177–189.
Thisarticleappliesbiometricalmodelingtoadolescentandyoungadultuseofalcohol.
DFanalysisisasimpleregression-basedapproachtoestimatinggeneticandenvironmentalvariancecomponents.
Jacobson,K.
C.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1999).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesontherelation-shipsbetweenfamilyconnectedness,schoolconnectedness,andadolescentdepressedmood:Sexdifferences.
DevelopmentalPsychology,35(4),926–939.
Thisarticleestimatesbiometricalmodelsforadolescentsrelatingfamily,school,andmentalhealth.
18JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
BardMcCartney,K.
,Harris,M.
J.
,&Bernieri,F.
(1990).
Growingupandgrowingapart:Adevelop-mentalmeta-analysisoftwinstudies.
PsychologicalBulletin,107,226–237.
Thisarticleisameta-analysisshowinghowbehaviorgeneticndingsrelatinggeneticandenvi-ronmentalinuencestovariousoutcomesmustbeconditionedontheageoftherespondent.
Itmotivatesthestudyofbehaviorgeneticpatternsinadolescentsaspotentiallydifferentthanforotheragegroups.
Plomin,R.
,&Rende,R.
(1991).
Humanbehavioralgenetics.
InM.
R.
Rosenzweig&L.
W.
Porter(Eds.
),AnnualReviewofPsychology(Vol.
42,pp.
161–190).
PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews.
Thisreviewarticleaccountsbroadlyforbiometrical/behaviorgeneticndingsacrossmanydiffer-entdomainsandages.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&Li,C.
(1994).
Beyondnatureversusnurture:DFanalysisofnonsharedinuencesonproblembehaviors.
DevelopmentalPsychology,30(3),374–384.
Thisarticlegoesbeyondtheusualpartitioningofinuencesingeneticandsharedenvironmentalinuences–amodelisdenedtoaccountexplicitlyformeasured,nonsharedenvironmentalinu-ences.
Themodelisappliedtothestudyofproblembehaviorsinolderchildhoodandyoungado-lescentrespondents.
Turkheimer,E.
(2000).
Threelawsofbehaviorgeneticsandwhattheymean.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalScience,9,160–164.
Thisarticlepresentssomeofthemethodologicalandempiricaldifcultiesofdoingresearchonbehaviorgenetics.
Thecaveatsthatemergeshouldinformallbehaviorgeneticmodelingefforts.
ReferencesAbdel-Rahim,A.
R.
,Nagoshi,C.
T.
,&Vandenberg,S.
G.
(1990).
Twinresemblancesincogni-tiveabilitiesinanEgyptiansample.
BehaviorGenetics,20(1),33–43.
Angoff,W.
H.
(1988).
Thenature–nurturedebate,aptitudes,andgroupdifferences.
AmericanPsychologist,43,713–720.
Belsky,J.
(2000).
Conditionalandalternativereproductivestrategies:Individualdifferencesinsus-ceptibilitytorearingexperiences.
InJ.
L.
Rodgers,D.
C.
Rowe,&W.
B.
Miller(Eds.
),Geneticinuencesonhumanfertilityandsexuality(pp.
127–168).
Boston:Kluwer.
Beunen,G.
,Maes,H.
H.
,Vlietinck,R.
,Malina,R.
M.
,Thomis,M.
,Feys,E.
,Loos,R.
,&Derom,C.
(1998).
Univariateandmultivariategeneticanalysisofsubcutaneousfatnessandfatdistributioninearlyadolescence.
BehaviorGenetics,28(4),279–288.
Billig,J.
P.
,Hershberger,S.
L.
,Iacono,W.
G.
,&McGue,M.
(1996).
Lifeeventsandpersonalityinlateadolescence:Geneticandenvironmentalrelations.
BehaviorGenetics,26(6),543–554.
Boomsma,D.
I.
,vandenBree,M.
B.
M.
,Orlebeke,J.
F.
,&Molenaar,P.
C.
M.
(1989).
Resem-blancesofparentsandtwinsinsportsparticipationandheartrate.
BehaviorGenetics,19(1),123–141.
Brown,B.
(1999).
Optimizingexpressionofthecommonhumangenomeforchilddevelopment.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalResearch,8,37–41.
Buchanan,C.
M.
,Eccles,J.
S.
,&Becker,J.
B.
(1992).
Areadolescentsthevictimsofraginghor-mones:Evidenceforactivationaleffectsofhormonesonmoodsandbehavioratadolescence.
PsychologicalBulletin,111,62–107.
Bussell,D.
A.
,Neiderhiser,J.
M.
,Pike,A.
,Plomin,R.
,Simmens,S.
,Howe,G.
W.
,BehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment19Hetherington,E.
M.
,Carroll,E.
,&Reiss,D.
(1999).
Adolescents'relationshipstosiblingsandmothers:Amultivariategeneticanalysis.
DevelopmentalPsychology,35(5),1248–1259.
Buster,M.
A.
,&Rodgers,J.
L.
(2000).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesonalcoholuse:DFanalysisofNLSYkinshipdata.
JournalofBiosocialScience,32,177–189.
Carey,G.
(1992).
Simulatedtwindataonsubstanceabuse.
BehaviorGenetics,22,193–196.
Doughty,D.
,&Rodgers,J.
L.
(2000).
BehaviorgeneticmodelingofmenarcheinU.
S.
females.
InJ.
L.
Rodgers,D.
C.
Rowe,&W.
B.
Miller(Eds.
),Geneticinuencesonhumanfertilityandsexuality(pp.
169–182).
Boston:Kluwer.
Dunne,M.
P.
,Martin,N.
G.
,Statham,D.
J.
,Slutske,W.
S.
,Dinwiddie,S.
H.
,Bucholz,K.
K.
,Madden,P.
A.
,&Heath,A.
C.
(1997).
Geneticandenvironmentalcontributionstovarianceinageatrstsexualintercourse.
PsychologicalScience,8,1–6.
Eaves,L.
,Martin,N.
,Heath,A.
,Schieken,R.
,Meyer,J.
,Silberg,J.
,Neale,M.
,&Corey,L.
(1997).
Agechangesinthecausesofindividualdifferencesinconservatism.
BehaviorGenetics,27(2),121–124.
Eaves,L.
J.
,Silberg,J.
L.
,Hewitt,J.
K.
,Rutter,M.
,Meyer,J.
M.
,Neale,M.
C.
,&Pickles,A.
(1993).
Analyzingtwinresemblanceinmultisymptomdata:Geneticapplicationofalatentclassmodelforsymptomsofconductdisorderinjuvenileboys.
BehaviorGenetics,23(1),5–19.
Elder,G.
E.
(1975).
Agedifferentiationandthelifecourse.
AnnualReviewofSociology,1,165–190.
Elkins,I.
J.
,McGue,M.
,&Iacono,W.
G.
(1997).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesonparent–sonrelationships:Evidenceforincreasinggeneticinuenceduringadolescence.
DevelopmentalPsychology,33(2),351–362.
Ensminger,M.
E.
(1987).
Adolescentsexualbehaviorasitrelatestoothertransitionbehaviorsinyouth.
InS.
Hofferth&C.
D.
Hayes(Eds.
),Riskingthefuture(Vol.
2,pp.
36–55).
Washington,DC:NationalAcademyPress.
Falconer,D.
S.
(1981).
Introductiontoquantitativegenetics.
NewYork:Longman.
Fisher,R.
A.
(1930).
Thegeneticaltheoryofnaturalselection.
Oxford:Clarendon.
Fulker,D.
W.
,Eysenck,S.
B.
G.
,&Zuckerman,M.
(1980).
Ageneticandenvironmentalanaly-sisofsensationseeking.
JournalofResearchonPersonality,14,261–281.
Gjone,H.
,Stevenson,J.
,Sundet,J.
M.
,&Eilertsen,D.
E.
(1996).
Changesinheritabilityacrossincreasinglevelsofbehaviorproblemsinyoungtwins.
BehaviorGenetics,26(4),419–426.
Gottlieb,G.
(2000).
Environmentalandbehavioralinuencesongeneactivity.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalResearch,9,93–97.
Grilo,C.
M.
,&Pogue-Geile,M.
R.
(1991).
Thenatureofenvironmentalinuencesonweightandobesity:Abehaviorgeneticanalysis.
PsychologicalBulletin,110,520–537.
Harris,J.
A.
,Vernon,P.
A.
,&Boomsma,D.
I.
(1998).
Theheritabilityoftestosterone:AstudyofDutchadolescenttwinsandtheirparents.
BehaviorGenetics,28(3),165–171.
Heath,A.
C.
,Kirk,K.
M.
,Meyer,J.
M.
,&Martin,N.
G.
(1999).
Geneticandsocialdetermi-nantsofinitiationandageatonsetofsmokinginAustraliantwins.
BehaviorGenetics,29,395–407.
Hogan,D.
P.
,&Astone,N.
M.
(1986).
Thetransitiontoadulthood.
AnnualReviewofSociology,12,109–130.
Houle,D.
(1992).
Comparingevolvabilityandvariabilityofquantitativetraits.
Genetics,130,195–204.
Jacobson,K.
C.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1998).
Geneticandsharedenvironmentalinuencesonadoles-centBMI:Interactionwithraceandsex.
BehaviorGenetics,28(4),265–278.
Jacobson,K.
C.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1999).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesontherelation-shipsbetweenfamilyconnectedness,schoolconnectedness,andadolescentdepressedmood:Sexdifferences.
DevelopmentalPsychology,35(4),926–939.
20JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
BardJessor,R.
,&Jessor,S.
L.
(1977).
Problembehaviorandpsychosocialdevelopment:Alongitudinalstudyofyouth.
NewYork:AcademicPress.
Kohler,H.
-P.
,Rodgers,J.
L.
,&Christensen,K.
(1999).
IsfertilitybehaviorinourgenesFind-ingsfromaDanishtwinstudy.
PopulationandDevelopmentReview,25,253–288.
Koopmans,J.
R.
,Boomsma,D.
I.
,Heath,A.
C.
,&vanDoornen,L.
J.
P.
(1995).
Amultivariategeneticanalysisofsensationseeking.
BehaviorGenetics,25(4),349–356.
Koopmans,J.
R.
,Slutske,W.
S.
,Heath,A.
C.
,Neale,M.
C.
,&Boomsma,D.
I.
(1999).
ThegeneticsofsmokinginitiationandquantitysmokedinDutchadolescentandyoungadulttwins.
BehaviorGenetics,29(6),383–393.
Lykken,D.
T.
,McGue,M.
,Tellegen,A.
,&Bouchard,T.
J.
(1992).
Emergenesis:Genetictraitsthatmaynotruninfamilies.
AmericanPsychologist,12,1565–1577.
Macaskill,G.
T.
,Hopper,J.
L.
,White,V.
,&Hill,D.
J.
(1994).
Geneticandenvironmentalvari-ationinEysenckPersonalityQuestionnairescalesmeasuredonAustralianadolescenttwins.
BehaviorGenetics,24,481–491.
Maccoby,E.
E.
(2000).
Parentinganditseffectsonchildren:Onreadingandmisreadingbehav-iorgenetics.
InS.
T.
Fiske,D.
L.
Schacter,&C.
Zahn-Waxler(Eds.
),AnnualReviewofPsychology(Vol.
51,pp.
1–27).
PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews.
Madden,P.
A.
F.
,Heath,A.
C.
,Pedersen,N.
L.
,Kaprio,J.
,Koskenvuo,M.
J.
,&Martin,N.
G.
(1999).
Thegeneticsofsmokingpersistenceinmenandwoman:Amulticulturalstudy.
BehaviorGenetics,29,423–431.
Mayhew,K.
P.
,Flay,B.
R.
,&Mott,J.
A.
(2000).
Stagesinthedevelopmentofadolescentsmoking.
DrugandAlcoholDependence,59(1),S61–S81.
McCartney,K.
,Harris,M.
J.
,&Bernieri,F.
(1990).
Growingupandgrowingapart:Adevelop-mentalmeta-analysisoftwinstudies.
PsychologicalBulletin,107,226–237.
McGue,M.
,Bacon,S.
,&Lykken,D.
T.
(1993).
Personalitystabilityandchangeinearlyadult-hood:Abehavioralgeneticanalysis.
DevelopmentalPsychology,29(1),96–109.
Meyer,J.
M.
,&Neale,M.
C.
(1992).
Therelationshipbetweenageatrstdruguseandteenagedruguseliability.
BehaviorGenetics,22(2),197–213.
Miller,W.
B.
,Pasta,D.
J.
,MacMurray,J.
,Chiu,C.
,Wu,H.
,&Comings,D.
E.
(1999).
Dopaminereceptorsareassociatedwithageatrstsexualintercourse.
JournalofBiosocialScience,31,91–97.
Moft,T.
E.
(1993).
Adolescence-limitedandlife-course-persistentantisocialbehavior:Adevel-opmentaltaxonomy.
PsychologicalReview,100,674–701.
Molenaar,P.
C.
M.
,Boomsma,D.
I.
,&Dolan,C.
V.
(1997).
Thedetectionofgenotype-environmentinteractioninlongitudinalgeneticmodels.
InM.
LaBuda,E.
Grigorenko,I.
Ravich-Scherbo,&S.
Scarr(Eds.
),Onthewaytoindividuality:Currentmethodologicalissuesinbehaviorgenetics(pp.
53–70).
Commack,NY:NovaScience.
Nagoshi,C.
T.
,&Johnson,R.
C.
(1993).
Familialtransmissionofcognitiveabilitiesinoffspringtestedinadolescenceandadulthood:Alongitudinalstudy.
BehaviorGenetics,23(3),279–285.
Neale,M.
C.
(1999).
Possibleconfoundsandtheirresolutioninmultivariategeneticanalyses:CommentonBusselletal.
(1999).
DevelopmentalPsychology,35(5),1260–1264.
Neale,M.
C.
,&Cardon,L.
R.
(1992).
Methodologyforgeneticstudiesoftwinsandfamilies.
London:Kluwer.
Neiderhiser,J.
M.
,Bussell,D.
A.
,Pike,A.
,Plomin,R.
,Simmens,S.
,Howe,G.
W.
,Hetherington,E.
M.
,&Reiss,D.
(1999).
Theimportanceofsharedenvironmentalinuencesinexplainingtheoverlapbetweenmother'sparentingandsiblingrelationships:ReplytoNeale(1999).
DevelopmentalPsychology,35(5),1265–1267.
Neiderhiser,J.
M.
,Pike,A.
,Hetherington,E.
M.
,&Reiss,D.
(1998).
AdolescentperceptionsasBehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment21mediatorsofparenting:Geneticandenvironmentalcontributions.
DevelopmentalPsychology,34(6),1459–1469.
O'Connor,T.
G.
,Deater-Deckard,K.
,Fulker,D.
,Rutter,M.
,&Plomin,R.
(1998).
Genotype-environmentcorrelationsinlatechildhoodandearlyadolescence:Antisocialbehavioralproblemsandcoerciveparenting.
DevelopmentalPsychology,34(5),970–981.
Petrill,S.
A.
,&Thompson,L.
A.
(1993).
Thephenotypicandgeneticrelationshipsamongmea-suresofcognitiveability,temperament,andscholasticachievement.
BehaviorGenetics,23(6),511–518.
Pike,A.
,McGuire,S.
,Hetherington,E.
M.
,Reiss,D.
,&Plomin,R.
(1996).
Familyenvironmentandadolescentdepressivesymptomsandantisocialbehavior:Amultivariategeneticanalysis.
DevelopmentalPsychology,32(4),590–603.
Plomin,R.
,Fulker,D.
W.
,Corley,R.
,&DeFries,J.
C.
(1997).
Nature,nurture,andcognitivedevelopmentfrom1to16years:Aparent–offspringadoptionstudy.
PsychologicalScience,8,442–447.
Plomin,R.
,Reiss,D.
,Hetherington,E.
M.
,&Howe,G.
W.
(1994).
Natureandnurture:Geneticcontributionstomeasureofthefamilyenvironment.
DevelopmentalPsychology,30(1),32–43.
Plomin,R.
,&Rende,R.
(1991).
Humanbehavioralgenetics.
InM.
R.
Rosenzweig&L.
W.
Porter(Eds.
),AnnualReviewofPsychology(Vol.
42,pp.
161–190).
PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews.
Rende,R.
D.
,Plomin,R.
,Reiss,D.
,&Hetherington,E.
M.
(1993).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesondepressivesymptomatologyinadolescence:Individualdifferencesandextremescores.
JournalofChildPsychologyandPsychiatry,34,1387–1398.
Rindfuss,R.
R.
,Swicegood,C.
G.
,&Rosenfeld,R.
A.
(1987).
Disorderinthelifecourse:HowcommonanddoesitmatterAmericanSociologicalReview,52,785–801.
Rodgers,J.
L.
(1996).
Sexualtransitionsinadolescence.
InJ.
A.
Graber,J.
Brooks-Gunn,&A.
C.
Petersen(Eds.
),Transitionsthroughadolescence:Interpersonaldomainsandcontext(pp.
85–108).
Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Billy,J.
O.
G.
,&Udry,J.
R.
(1984).
Amodeloffriendshipsimilarityinmildlydeviantbehaviors.
JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology,14,413–425.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,&Buster,M.
(1994).
SeasonalityofmenarcheamongU.
S.
females:Correlatesandlinkages.
InK.
L.
Campbell&J.
W.
Wood,Humanreproductiveecology:Interactionsofenvironment,fertility,andbehavior.
NewYork:NewYorkAcademyofSciences.
Rodgers,J.
L.
&Doughty,D.
(2000).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesonfertilityexpecta-tionsandoutcomesusingNLSYkinshipdata.
InJ.
L.
Rodgers,D.
C.
Rowe,&W.
B.
Miller(Eds.
),Geneticinuencesonhumanfertilityandsexuality(pp.
85–106).
Boston:Kluwer.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Hughes,K.
,Kohler,H.
-P.
,Christensen,K.
,Doughty,D.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&Miller,W.
B.
(2001).
Geneticinuencehelpsexplainvariationinhumanfertilityoutcomes:Evidencefromrecentbehavioralandmoleculargeneticstudies.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalScience,inpress.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1990).
Adolescentsexualactivityandmildlydeviantbehavior:Siblingandfriendshipeffects.
JournalofFamilyIssues,11,274–293.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1993).
Socialcontagionandadolescentsexualbehavior:Adevel-opmentalEMOSAmodel.
PsychologicalReview,100,479–510.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&Buster,M.
(1999).
Nature,nurture,andrstsexualintercourseintheUSA:FittingbehavioralgeneticmodelstoNLSYkinshipdata.
JournalofBiosocialScience,31,29–41.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&Li,C.
(1994).
Beyondnatureversusnurture:DFanalysisofnonsharedinuencesonproblembehaviors.
DevelopmentalPsychology,30(3),374–384.
Rodgers,J.
L.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&May,K.
(1994).
DFanalysisofNLSYIQ/achievementdata:Nonsharedenvironmentalinuences.
Intelligence,19,157–177.
22JosephLeeRodgersandDavidE.
BardRodgers,J.
L.
,Rowe,D.
C.
,&Miller,W.
B.
(2000).
Geneticinuencesonhumanfertilityandsexuality:Theoreticalandempiricalcontributionsfromthebiologicalandbehavioralsciences.
Boston:Kluwer.
Rose,R.
J.
(1995).
Genesandhumanbehavior.
InJ.
T.
Spence,J.
M.
Darley,&D.
J.
Foss(Eds.
),AnnualReviewofPsychology(Vol.
46,pp.
625–654).
PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews.
Rowe,D.
C.
,Almeida,D.
M.
,&Jacobson,K.
C.
(1999).
Schoolcontextandgeneticinuencesonaggressioninadolescence.
PsychologicalScience,10,277–280.
Rowe,D.
C.
,Chassin,L.
,Presson,L.
,&Sherman,S.
J.
(1996).
Parentalsmokingandtheepidemicspreadofcigarettesmoking.
JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology,26(5),437–454.
Rowe,D.
C.
,&Plomin,R.
(1981).
Theimportanceofnonshared(E1)environmentalinuencesinbehavioraldevelopment.
DevelopmentalPsychology,17,517–531.
Rowe,D.
C.
,&Rodgers,J.
L.
(1991).
Adolescentsmokinganddrinking:AretheyepidemicsJournalofStudiesonAlcohol,52(2),110–117.
Steinberg,L.
,&Morris,A.
S.
(2001).
Adolescentdevelopment.
InS.
T.
Fiske,D.
L.
Schacter,&C.
Zahn-Waxler(Eds.
),AnnualReviewofPsychology(Vol.
52,pp.
83–110).
PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews.
Susman,E.
J.
,Nottelmann,E.
D.
,Inoff-Germain,G.
E.
,Dorn,L.
D.
,Cutler,G.
B.
,Loriaux,D.
L.
,&Chrousos,G.
P.
(1985).
Therelationshipofrelativehormonelevelsandphysicaldevel-opmentandsocio-emotionalbehaviorinyoungadolescents.
JournalofYouthandAdolescence,14,245–264.
Topolski,T.
D.
,Hewitt,J.
K.
,Eaves,L.
J.
,Silberg,J.
L.
,Meyer,J.
M.
,Rutter,M.
,Pickles,A.
,&Simonoff,E.
(1997).
Geneticandenvironmentalinuencesonchildreportsofmanifestanxietyandoveranxiousdisorders:Acommunity-basedtwinstudy.
BehaviorGenetics,27,15–28.
Turkheimer,E.
(1998).
Heritabilityandbiologicalexplanation.
PsychologicalReview,105,782–791.
Turkheimer,E.
(2000).
Threelawsofbehaviorgeneticsandwhattheymean.
CurrentDirectionsinPsychologicalScience,9,160–164.
Turkheimer,E.
,&Waldron,M.
(2000).
Nonsharedenvironment:Atheoretical,methodological,andquantitativereview.
PsychologicalBulletin,126,78–108.
Udry,J.
R.
,Billy,J.
O.
G.
,Morris,N.
M.
,Groff,T.
R.
,&Raj,M.
H.
(1985).
Serumandrogenichormonesmotivatesexualbehaviorinadolescentboys.
FertilityandSterility,43,90–94.
Udry,J.
R.
,&Talbert,L.
M.
(1988).
Sexhormoneeffectsonpersonalityatpuberty.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,54,291–295.
Udry,J.
R.
,Talbert,L.
M.
,&Morris,N.
M.
(1986).
Biosocialfoundationsforfemaleadolescentactivity.
Demography,23,217–230.
vandenOord,E.
J.
C.
G.
,Boomsma,D.
I.
,&Verhulst,F.
C.
(1994).
Astudyofproblembehav-iorsin10-to15-year-oldbiologicallyrelatedandunrelatedinternationaladoptees.
BehaviorGenetics,24(3),193–205.
vandenOord,E.
J.
C.
G.
,&Rowe,D.
C.
(1997).
Continuityandchangeinchildren'ssocialmaladjustment:Adevelopmentalbehaviorgeneticstudy.
DevelopmentalPsychology,33(2),319–332.
vanderValk,J.
C.
,Verhulst,F.
C.
,Neale,M.
C.
,&Boomsma,D.
I.
(1998).
Longitudinalgeneticanalysisofproblembehaviorsinbiologicallyrelatedandunrelatedadoptees.
BehaviorGenetics,28(5),365–380.
Viken,R.
J.
,Kaprio,J.
,Koskenvuo,M.
,&Rose,R.
J.
(1999).
Longitudinalanalysesofthedeter-minantsofdrinkingandofdrinkingtointoxicationinadolescenttwins.
BehaviorGenetics,29,455–461.
BehaviorGeneticsandAdolescentDevelopment23

易探云香港vps主机价格多少钱?香港云服务器主机租用价格

易探云香港vps主机价格多少钱?香港vps主机租用费用大体上是由配置决定的,我们选择香港vps主机租用最大的优势是免备案vps。但是,每家服务商的机房、配置、定价也不同。我们以最基础配置为标准,综合比对各大香港vps主机供应商的价格,即可选到高性能、价格适中的香港vps主机。通常1核CPU、1G内存、2Mbps独享带宽,价格在30元-120元/月。不过,易探云香港vps主机推出四个机房的优惠活动,...

spinservers($179/月),1Gbps不限流量服务器,双E5-2630Lv3/64GB/1.6T SSD/圣何塞机房

中秋节快到了,spinservers针对中国用户准备了几款圣何塞机房特别独立服务器,大家知道这家服务器都是高配,这次推出的机器除了配置高以外,默认1Gbps不限制流量,解除了常规机器10TB/月的流量限制,价格每月179美元起,机器自动化上架,一般30分钟内,有基本自助管理功能,带IPMI,支持安装Windows或者Linux操作系统。配置一 $179/月CPU:Dual Intel Xeon E...

ReadyDedis:VPS全场5折,1G内存套餐月付2美元起,8个机房可选_服务器安装svn

ReadyDedis是一家2018年成立的国外VPS商家,由印度人开设,主要提供VPS和独立服务器租用等,可选数据中心包括美国洛杉矶、西雅图、亚特兰大、纽约、拉斯维加斯、杰克逊维尔、印度和德国等。目前,商家针对全部VPS主机提供新年5折优惠码,优惠后最低套餐1GB内存每月仅需2美元起,所有VPS均为1Gbps端口不限流量方式。下面列出几款主机配置信息。CPU:1core内存:1GB硬盘:25GB ...

169pp com为你推荐
google地球打不开手机谷歌地球怎么打不开?百度手写百度为什么没有了在线手写输入法手机区号手机电话号码开头95共15位号码是什么手机号码?申请证书手机申请证书直播加速有没有软件使已经下载好了的视频播放加速,例如30分钟的视频15分钟或者20分钟播放完xv播放器下载求手机可以看xv格式的视频播放器数据库损坏数据库坏了,怎么修复?怎么升级ios6苹果6怎么升级最新系统宕机何谓宕机?网络广告投放网络广告的投放目的是什么?
租服务器 187邮箱 winhost mediafire下载工具 suspended 外国空间 天猫双十一抢红包 京东商城双十一活动 ibox官网 100m免费空间 e蜗 qingyun 免空 微信收钱 卡巴斯基试用版 上海联通宽带测速 多线空间 免费mysql数据库 登陆空间 阿里云官方网站 更多