RESEARCHARTICLEOpenAccessCommunicationpatternsinapsychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjury:AquantitativecasestudybasedonsymbolicdynamicsPaulERapp1*,ChristopherJCellucci2,AdeleMKGilpin3,4,MiguelAJiménez-Montao5andKathrynEKorslund6AbstractBackground:Theroleofpsychotherapyinthetreatmentoftraumaticbraininjuryisreceivingincreasedattention.
Theevaluationofpsychotherapywiththesepatientshasbeenconductedlargelyintheabsenceofquantitativedataconcerningthetherapyitself.
Quantitativemethodsforcharacterizingthesequence-sensitivestructureofpatient-therapistcommunicationarenowbeingdevelopedwiththeobjectiveofimprovingtheeffectivenessofpsychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
Methods:Thecontentofthreetherapysessiontranscripts(sessionswereseparatedbyfourmonths)obtainedfromapatientwithahistoryofseveralmotorvehicleaccidentswhowasreceivingdialecticalbehaviortherapywasscoredandanalyzedusingmethodsderivedfromthemathematicaltheoryofsymbolicdynamics.
Results:Theanalysisofsymbolfrequencieswaslargelyuninformative.
Whenrepeatedtripleswereexaminedamarkedpatternofchangeincontentwasobservedoverthethreesessions.
ThecontextfreegrammarcomplexityandtheLempel-Zivcomplexitywerecalculatedforeachtherapysession.
Forbothmeasures,therateofcomplexitygeneration,expressedasbitsperminute,increasedlongitudinallyduringthecourseoftherapy.
Thebetween-sessionincreasesincomplexitygenerationratesareconsistentwithcalculationsofmutualinformation.
Takentogethertheseresultsindicatethattherewasaquantifiableincreaseinthevariabilityofpatient-therapistverbalbehaviorduringthecourseoftherapy.
Comparisonofcomplexityvaluesagainstvaluesobtainedfromequiprobablerandomsurrogatesestablishedthepresenceofanonrandomstructureinpatient-therapistdialog(P=.
002).
Conclusions:Whilerecognizingthatonlylimitedconclusionscanbebasedonacasehistory,itcanbenotedthatthesequantitativeobservationsareconsistentwithqualitativeclinicalobservationsofincreasesintheflexibilityofdiscourseduringtherapy.
Theseprocedurescanbeofparticularvalueintheexaminationoftherapiesfollowingtraumaticbraininjurybecause,insomepresentations,thesetherapiesarecomplicatedbydeficitsthatresultinsubtledistortionsoflanguagethatproducesignificantpost-injurysocialimpairment.
Independentlyofthemathematicalanalysisappliedtotheinvestigationoftherapy-generatedsymbolsequences,ourexperiencesuggeststhattheprocedurespresentedhereareofvalueintrainingtherapists.
Keywords:traumaticbraininjury,psychotherapy,psychoanalysis,complexity,mutualinformation,entropy,infor-mationtheory,symbolicdynamics*Correspondence:prapp@usuhs.
mil1DepartmentofMilitaryandEmergencyMedicine,UniformedServicesUniversity,4301JonesBridgeRoad,Bethesda,MD20814,USAFulllistofauthorinformationisavailableattheendofthearticleRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/1192011Rappetal;licenseeBioMedCentralLtd.
ThisisanOpenAccessarticledistributedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense(http://creativecommons.
org/licenses/by/2.
0),whichpermitsunrestricteduse,distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalworkisproperlycited.
BackgroundTraumaticbraininjuryisasignificantcauseofacuteandlong-termdisability.
Neurobehavioralsequelaeencom-passcognitive,socialandpsychiatricdomains.
Majordepressivedisorderisthemostprevalentpsychiatricdis-orderfollowingtraumaticbraininjuryregardlessoftheseverityoftheinjury[1-9].
Estimatesofprevalencearehighlyvaried.
Iverson,etal.
[10]reviewedsixstudiesofdepressionfollowingtraumaticbraininjuryandfoundreportsofprevalencerangingfrom12%to44%.
Whileprevalenceratesareuncertain,acriticalconclusioncanbemade.
Thetreatmentofneuropsychiatricdisordersfollowingtraumaticbraininjuryisasignificantclinicalneedthatpresentsuniqueclinicalchallenges.
Ascommonlyconceptualized,theclinicalresponsetotraumaticbraininjuryhasfourcomponents:neuropro-tection(preserveinjuredneurons),plasticmodification(reconstructneuralnetworkswithsurvivingneuronsbypromotingdendriticarborizationandsynaptogenesis),neurogenesis(stimulatethematurationofnewneuronsfromstemcellpopulations),andneurointegration(facili-tatetheintegrationofnewlyformedneuronsintothecentralnervoussystem).
Itisincreasinglyrecognized,however,thatpsychotherapyisanimportantcomple-menttothisneurologicalresponse.
Cope[11]hasarguedthat"themajorityofrecoveringsurvivorsofTBIarenowseenaspotentiallybenefitingfromsomeformofpsy-chotherapeutic/rehabilitationtreatment.
"Nonetheless,mostindividualsexperiencingaheadinjurydonotreceivepsychotherapy.
InareviewoftheearlyhistoryofpsychotherapyfollowingTBI,Prigatano[12]addressedthequestion,"WhyhastheroleofpsychotherapeuticinterventionsintherehabilitationcareofTBIpatientsgoneunrecognized"Hesuggeststhat"theanswerseemstolieintheassumptionthatTBIpatientscouldnotbene-fitfrompsychotherapybecauseoftheirpermanentcogni-tive,linguisticandaffectivedisturbances.
"WhilethisargumentmightbeadvancedwhenconsideringsevereTBI,itdoesnotseemplausibleincasesofmildTBI.
ButisitevenapplicableinthecaseofsevereTBIResultsreportedbyBen-Yishayetal.
[13]andbyEzrachi,etal.
[14]indicatethatpsychotherapyfollowingmoderateorsevereTBIhasapositiveeffectonpost-injuryemployment.
Whilepsychotherapyisthepreferredapproachtothetreatmentofmooddisordersfollowingtraumaticbraininjury[1,2,15-17]thereislimitedresearchtohelpguidetheselectionofthespecifictherapeuticmethod[18,19].
Theheterogeneityofthispopulationdemandsavariedresponse.
Inpart,theappropriatetherapywillbedeter-minedbythephysicalinjury,particularlytheresidualneu-rologicalandcognitivedeficits.
IndividualswithTBImaybenefitfromtreatmentsthattakepost-injurycognitivedistortionsintoaccount[20-22].
Thechoiceoftherapyshouldalsoberesponsivetopre-injurypsychopathology[23,24].
Thereisanemergingliteraturedetailingthebene-fitsofcognitivebehaviortherapyacrossavarietyofmedi-calpatientswithacquiredbraininjuriesofvariousseveritiescomorbidwithmooddisorders[15-18,25,26].
Psychodynamicpsychotherapyhasalsobeenconsidered.
Whilecognitivedeficitsfollowingheadinjurycanlimittheindividual'sabilitytoprofitfrompsychodynamicpsy-chotherapy,thisisnotinvariablythecase.
AsLewisandRosenberg[27]observedinapaperdescribingpsychoana-lyticpsychotherapyfollowingbraininjury,"theoverridingprinciplethatguidessuchpsychotherapeuticworkisthatacquiredbrainlesionsdonotablatethepatient'spsycheorunconscious.
"Theseauthorshaveidentifiedfivecriteriathatcanhelpidentifycandidatesforpsychoanalyticpsy-chotherapyfollowingbraininjury.
(1.
)Thepatientmustbemotivatedtoenterandremainintherapy.
(2.
)Patientswhohavehadatleastonepositivesignificantrelationshipinthepastarebetterabletoformatherapeuticalliance.
(3.
)Patientswhohavehadprevioussuccessesresultingfromactiveeffortaremorelikelytobenefitfromindivi-dualtherapy.
(4.
)Patientsinextremepsychologicaldistressmayrequireamoresupportiveintervention,includinghospitalization,beforeinitiatingpsychoanalytictherapy.
(5.
)Thedegreeandformofbraininjurycanaffecttheappropriatenessofanalytictreatment.
Patientswithsignifi-cantexpressiveorreceptivelanguagedeficitsarenotappropriatecandidates.
Inadditiontooutliningthepoten-tialbenefitsofapsychodynamicallyorientedtherapyforappropriatelyselectedpatients,LewisandRosenbergmaketwopointsthataregenericallyapplicabletotheconsidera-tionofpsychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
First,unaddressedpsychologicalproblemscanbeanimpe-dimenttomeaningfulparticipationinphysical,cognitiveandoccupationalrehabilitation,thusprovidinganaddi-tionalargumentforincludingpsychotherapyinthetreat-mentofsomepresentationsoftraumaticbraininjury.
Second,thepatient'salteredexperienceofselfshouldnotbeviewedasanentirelyneurologicalsymptom.
Braininju-rieshavepsychologicalmeaning.
"Althoughsuchdisruptions(braininjury)cansignifi-cantlyaffectthepatient'sself-esteem,andoftenrepre-sentamajorfocusforfamilywork,theymayrepresentamorebasicandprofounddisturbanceinthepatients'senseofself.
Thatis,beyondtheirdifficultiesinper-formingsocialroles,patientsalsostrugglewiththemorefundamentalquestionofwhotheyare;thebraininjuryappearstodisruptseverelytheirpreviouslyacquiredself-imageandsenseofself[28].
Thus,aprimarytaskofpsychotherapyistohelpthepatientconsolidateanewsenseofselfthatsuccessfullyRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page2of28incorporatesarealisticappraisalofstrengthsandweak-nesses"[27].
Inpresentationswherethisalterationofsenseofselfisasignificantelementoftheclinicalpresentationandthepatienthassufficientegodevelopmenttotolerateaninsightdirectedtherapy,apsychodynamicallyinformedtherapyisindicated.
OnreviewingpsychotherapiesappropriateforTBIpatients,Folzer[29]madethefollowingobservation,"If'immature'defensesandcopingpatternsareremovedtooearly,thetherapistmayprecipitateacatastrophe.
Insteadofdirectlyconfrontingthepatient,thetherapistcanintro-ducethefocusonrealitygradually.
"Thiswouldargueforasupportivetherapy[30]insteadofinsight-orientedther-apy.
Thereisnot,however,astrictdivisionbetweentheseformsoftherapy.
AsWerman[30]observed,"Althoughinthefollowingpagesthesetwoformsoftreatment(suppor-tivetherapyandinsight-orientedtherapy)arecomparedasiftheywerenotonlydifferentfromeachotherbutvir-tuallydichotomousintheiraimsandtechniques,inrealitytheyrarelyexistinpureforms.
Typically,overaperiodoftime,mostpatientsinsupportivepsychotherapygainsomeinsightintotheirbehavior;similarlyitisdifficulttoconceiveofacourseofinsight-orientedpsychotherapyinwhichsomesupportivemeasuresarenotutilized.
"Psychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjuryshouldnotnecessarilybelimitedtoindividualtherapy.
SeveralauthorshaveemphasizedthevalueofgrouptherapywithTBIpatients[29,31,32],andfamilyinvolvementintherapycanbeparticularlyimportant[12,23].
ThediscussionofpsychotherapywithTBIpatientsandindeedpsychotherapyingeneralhasbeenconductedlargelyintheabsenceofquantitativedataconcerningthetherapyitself.
Whilestandardizedinstrumentsforasses-singbaselinesymptomsandtreatmentoutcomesareincreasinglybeingusedinclinicalresearch[33],theseinstrumentsdonotquantifythefinestructureofthether-apeuticinteraction.
Thiscontributioncontinuesthedevel-opmentofquantitativemethodsforthecharacterizationofpatient-therapistcommunicationwiththelongtermobjectiveofimprovingtheeffectivenessofpsychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
Communicationbetweenpatientsandtherapistduringpsychotherapyhasmanycomponentsincludingposture,eyecontact,verbaltone,verbalproduction(thenumberofwordsexchangedirre-spectiveoftheirmeaning)andthemanifestcontentofthecommunication.
Alloftheseinteractionscanbeexaminedquantitatively[34,35].
Forexamplenon-verbalcommuni-cationinthetherapist-patientinteractionhasbeenana-lyzedbyYaynal-Reymond,etal.
[36]andbyMertenandSchwab[37]usingaformofquantificationdevelopedbyMagnusson[38,39].
Whileallcomponentsofpatient-therapistcommunicationareimportant,thispaperfocusesoncontentanalysis.
Usingmethodsofsymbolicdynamicsthisinvestigationextendspreviousanalysesofthefrequencyofcontentbyquantifyingthetemporallydepen-dent,sequence-sensitivestructureofthedialog.
Aslong-termgoals,thequestionsaddressedinthisresearchprogramfollowthoseenumeratedinRapp,etal.
[40].
1.
Aretherenonrandompatternsinthesequentialstructureofpatient-therapistcommunication2.
Dothesepatterns,shouldtheyexist,changeduringthecourseoftherapy3.
Dochangesinthepatternsofpatient-therapistcommunicationcorrelatewiththeclinicallyperceivedsuccessorfailureofthetherapy4.
CanthistypeofanalysisidentifymoreeffectiveformsoftherapistinterventionThiscasestudyislimitedtoanexaminationofthefirstthreequestionsinthreetherapysessionsrecordedfromonepatient.
Generalizedconclusionscannotthereforebemade.
Thelimitedresultsdo,however,indicatethatthereisanonrandomstructureinpatient-therapistcommunica-tionintheseprotocols.
Additionally,quantifiablestruc-tureschangedduringthecourseoftherapyinamannerthatcorrelatedwiththeclinicallyperceivedsuccessofthetherapy.
Quantitativeinvestigationsofpatient-therapistcommunication:PriorResearchAfirstapproachtoquantitativecontentanalysisisthedeterminationofwordfrequency.
AnearlyeffortwasElec-tronicVerbalAnalysis[41]measuringthefrequencyofanxietyrelatedwords.
Inasubsequentstudy,Pennebaker,etal.
[42,43]recordedthefrequencyof2800wordsthatwereplacedintosevencategories,andHart[44]analyzedpoliticaltextswithalibraryof10,000wordsinfiveclasseswithapproximatelysevencategoriesineachclass.
Thelimitationsoftheseanalysesareclear.
Wordfrequencyisinsensitivetocontext.
Arandomlyshuffledtextwillpro-ducedthesamewordcounts.
AsFastandFunder[45]observe,forexample,thephrase"Iamnothappy"maybescoredaspositiveemotionalcontent.
Severalinvestigatorshavedevelopedmethodsthatmovebeyondwordfrequencytoexaminemeaning.
ApioneerinthiseffortwasHartvigDahlwhoseinvestiga-tionofthecaseofMrs.
Canalyzed1,114psychoanalyticsessionswiththesamepatient[46-48].
Inthe1974study[48],entriesinathreethousandworddictionarywereassignedtoonedenotativecategoryandtooneormoreconnotativecategories.
Afactoranalysiswasusedtoidentifygroupsofrelatedwords,anditwasshownthatthesegroupswererelatedtothemespresentinthetran-script.
In1978Dahl,etal.
[49]publishedanapplicationoflinguisticanalysisinpsychotherapythatisintermedi-atetoanalysisofwordcountandtheanalysisofsequen-tialstructurebasedonsymbolicdynamicspresentedinRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page3of28thenextsection.
Inthisstudy,theanalysiswaslimitedtoanexaminationofthetherapist'sinterventions.
Thispro-videsaninstructiveandvaluablealternativetotheprac-ticeofconsideringonlythepatient'sspeech.
Eachinterventionbythetherapistwasclassifiedbytypeandratedonscalesdesignedtoassesscountertransferencemanifestations,includinghostility,seductiveness,approval,disapprovalandassertionofauthority.
Aquali-tativelinguisticanalysisbasedonChomsky'smodeloftransformationalgrammar[50,51]wasalsoimplemented.
Dahlandhiscolleagueshypothesizedthat"aspeakerhasavailableavarietyofsyntacticoptions,andtheparticularsyntacticstructurewhichhechoosesreflects,amongotherthings,theinventoryofwishesthatheismotivatedbothtoconcealandtoexpress.
"Theanalysisofexamplespresentedinthispapershowsoccasionsofextraposition,pseudocleftconstruction,syntacticambiguityandlexicalambiguityconsistentwiththishypothesis.
IntheGottschalk-Gleseranalysisprocedure[52,53],thegrammaticalclauseistheunitofanalysis.
Contentisscoredonsevenscales.
Inadditiontothestudyofpsy-chotherapy,Gottschalk-Gleserconstantanalysishasbeenappliedinmedicalpsychology[54-58].
GBSoft-waremarketsasoftwareproduct,PCAD2000,thatappliesaGottschalk-Glesercontentanalysistomachinereadabletext.
Inadditiontoderivingscoresonsevenscales,theprogramoffersaneuropsychiatricclassifica-tionbasedontheDSM-IV.
Langsandcolleagues[40,59]analyzedeachelementoftherapytranscriptsonfourteendimensions.
Theresultisacontentmatrixoffourteencolumns.
Theanalysisincludedcalculationsofthefrequencyofeachentry,Shannoninformationofeachcolumnandthecontextfreegrammarcomplexity(Jiménez-Montao,[60]describedinthenextsectionofthispaperandinAppen-dixOne).
Inthe1991study[40],twoone-hourprotocolsobtainedfromthesamepatientwithdifferenttherapistwereanalyzedbythisprocedure.
Onetherapistwasaclassicallytrainedpsychoanalyst.
TheothertherapistusedacommunicativeapproachdevelopedbyLangs[61].
Themostnotabledifferencesbetweenthetwopro-tocolswerethefrequencyofscoresforthevariablechar-acterizingthesphereofreference(1=therapyrelated,2=situationsoutsideoftherapy,3=referencetotherapyandsituationsoutsideoftherapy,4=unclear).
Inthecaseoftheanalyst,90%ofthematerialreferredtositua-tionsoutsideoftherapyandlessthan1%referredtother-apyrelatedissues.
Inthecaseofthecommunicativetherapist,20%ofthematerialfocusedonthetherapeuticsituation.
Giventhefocusonthepatient-therapistrela-tionshipincommunicativepsychotherapy,thisobserva-tionisconsistentwiththerapistexpectations.
StilesVerbalModeAnalysis[62-64]couldbedescribedasastatementclassificationmethod.
Theunitanalyzedisan"utterance"(definedpresently).
Eachunitiscodedintooneofeightclassesbyasequenceofthreeforced-choicequestions.
Eightverbalresponsemodesresult.
Theanalysiscontinueswithacalculationofthefrequencyofeachclass.
VerbalModeAnalysisisconsideredatgreaterlengthintheDiscussionsectionofthispaper.
Investigatorshavealsoexaminedthenarrativespeechofclinicalpopulationsusingsymbolicdynamics.
Incon-trastwiththeresearchdescribedabove,thesestudiesdonotexaminepatienttherapistcommunication.
Rathertheyexaminethesequence-sensitivestructureofcontin-uousnarrativeselicitedbythequestion,"Canyoutellmethestoryofyourlife"[65,66]oranarrativepro-ducedbyaparticipantinresponsetoarequesttorecallthecontentofastorythattheyhavejustread[67].
TheLeroy,etal.
[67]studyinvestigatedthesequence-sensitivestructureofarecallnarrativepresentedbyschi-zophrenicpatients.
FollowingKintschandVanDijk[68,69],theparticipant'snarrativewastreatedasasequenceofpropositions.
TheKintschandVanDijkdefi-nitionofapropositionistheminimalsemanticunitthatcanbeeithertrueorfalse.
Propositionswereclassifiedasmacro-propositionsthatspecifythetopicofdiscourseormicro-propositionsthatprovidedetails.
Macro-proposi-tionswereassignedthesymbol"M,"andmicro-proposi-tionswereassignedthesymbol"m.
"thenarrativesamplewasthusrecastasanorderedsequenceofM'sandm's.
Entropy,Lempel-Zivcomplexityandthefirstordertransi-tionmatrixwerecalculated.
Comparisonswithsurrogatedataestablishedthepresenceofasequence-sensitivenon-randomstructureinthedata.
Theglobalcomplexityofrecalldidnotdifferforcontrolandschizophrenicpartici-pants.
Therewas,however,adifferenceinthetransitionstructure.
Thereweremoremicro-propositionstomicro-propositiontransitionsinschizophrenicnarratives.
InDoba,etal.
[65]autobiographicalspeechofanorexicswasparsedinto5secondepochs.
Eachepochwasassignedoneoffoursymbolscorrespondingtonegativeemotion,positiveemotion,neutralemotionandsilence.
Inadditiontodistribution-determinedmeasures,theLempel-Zivcomplexityandthefirstordertransitionmatrixwereexamined.
Complexitycalculationswithsurrogatedataestablishedthepresenceofanon-randomsequentialstructureinthenarratives.
Inanorexics,dynamicalmea-suresidentifiedrecurrentcyclesbetweenexpressionsofnegativeemotionandsilencethatwerelessprominentinthecontrolpopulation.
Inasubsequentstudy[66],thesametranscriptswereanalyzedwithadifferentscoringsystem.
Fivesymbolswereused(familyrelations,socialrelations,loverelations,self-referenceandsilence).
Calcu-lationofmutualinformationwiththeoriginalsymbolsequencesandsurrogatedatasetsagainestablishedthepresenceofanon-randomdynamicalstructureintheRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page4of28narrative.
Theexaminationofthesummedprobabilitycurrents,ameasurederivedfromthefirstordertransitionmatrix,demonstratedthatthenarrativesofanorexicsareclosertostatisticalequilibriumthanthenarrativesofcontrols.
MethodsPatientHistoryInthisstudy,wedescribetheanalysisofthreetherapysessions(eachseparatedbyfourmonths)conductedwiththesamepatient(female,32yearsofage)bythesametherapist(female).
Thepatienthadexperiencedseveraltraumaticbraininjuresinasequenceofmotorvehicleaccidentstwoyearspriortotheinitiationoftherapy.
Thepatientwasreferredbyherpsychiatristfortargetedpsychotherapytreatmentofpre-existing,non-suicidalself-injuryandsevereemotionaldysregulation.
Neurologicalexaminationestablishedtheabsenceofresidualneurologicaldeficitspriortotheinitiationoftherapy.
Theaccidenthistorywas,however,deemedtobepsychologicallysignificantandhadacontinuingnegativeimpactonthepatient'srelationshipwithherpartner.
Thepatientreceivedweeklyindividualoutpati-enttherapyandgroupdeliveredtraininginbehavioralskills.
Theanalyzedsessionswerefromtheindividualtherapycomponent.
Eachsessionwassixtyminuteslong.
AtthetimeofinitiationoftreatmentthepatientmetDSM-IVdiagnosticcriteriaforborderlinepersonal-itydisorder.
ThisdiagnosiswasconfirmedwithaSCID-II(StructuredClinicalInterviewforDiagnosis)assess-ment.
ThepatientwasindialecticalbehaviortherapyfollowingthemethodsdevelopedbyLinehan[70,71].
Treatmentwasongoingbetweenthesessionscoded.
InstitutionalReviewBoardandtheparticipant'sinformedconsentwereobtainedpriortoinitiationofthestudy.
Therapysessionswerevideotapedforsubse-quentanalysis.
AnassessmentbasedontheDSM-IVwasrepeatedattheendoftreatmentatwhichtimethepatientnolongermetclinicalcriteriaforadiagnosisofborderlinepersonalitydisorder.
Selfreportratingsofmisery,depressedmoodandanxietywereimproved.
Indicesthatbroughtthepatienttotreatment,frequentsuicidalideationandrepeatedself-injury,werenolongerpresentandwerenotpresentatpost-treatmentfollow-upsixmonthsaftertheterminationoftherapy.
RestatementoftheProtocolasaSymbolSequenceThereareseveralpossibleproceduresforparsingather-apyprotocolpriortorestatementasasymbolsequence.
Onepossibilityistosetafixedtimeintervalandcodethecontentofthatinterval.
ThiswastheprocedurefollowedbyDoba,etal.
[65,66]whoused5secondintervalsintheiranalysisofautobiographicalspeech.
Whilehavingtheadvantageofexplicitspecification,thisprocedurehasthedisadvantageofbeingnonresponsivetothevaryingpaceofnaturaldialog.
Weimplementedherethemorecommonpractice,followingStiles[62-64,72]ofparsingtheprotocolintonaturalspeechelements.
Theseele-mentsarecalledutterancesinthetechnicalliterature.
AsdefinedbyStiles,etal.
[72]"Thecodingunitforbothformsandintentistheutterance,definedasanindepen-dentclause,nonrestrictivedependentclause,multiplepredicate,ortermofacknowledgment,evaluationoraddress.
"AdetailedpresentationofthedefinitionofanutterancewhichincludesexamplesisgiveninChapter8ofStiles'book"DescribingTalk"[62].
EachunitoftheprotocolwasassignedoneormoresymbolsusingthescoringsystemshowninTable1.
Theprotocolwasthusreducedtoasequenceofsymbolsdrawnformatwenty-twosymbolalphabet(Therapist:A,B,C,.
.
.
K,Patient:a,b,c,.
.
.
k)asshowninTable1.
Thissymbolsetwaschosentoemphasizeelementsthatareprominentinapsychotherapyofborderlinepersonalitydisorderbasedondialecticalbehaviortherapy[70,71].
Patientandtherapistcontentwasscoredforallthreeses-sions.
Inthispreliminarycasestudyparsingintoutter-ancesandsymbolassignmentwasaccomplishedbythecollectivedecisionofthreeinvestigators.
Itisrecognizedthatamoresystematicinvestigationwillrequireindepen-dentassessmentandaquantitativetestofinter-raterreliability.
Thefollowinggivesanexampleofeachcon-tenttype.
Acknowledging:"Thankyouforremindingmeofthat.
"Information(requesting/providing):"I'vehadthatcarfortwoyears.
"RequestforValidation:"WasIwrongtothinkthatway"Table1ProtocolScoringProcedureTherapistPatientContentAaAcknowledgingBbInformation(Requesting/Providing)CcRequestforValidationDdValidatingEeEmotionalDischargeFfComplaintGgTransitional/elicitationHhProblemPresentationIiBehavioralAnalysis/EducationalJjReflectiveKkIrreverentAsymbolisassignedtospecificcontentelements.
Uppercasesymbolswereusedwhenthetherapistwasspeaking,andlowercasesymbolswereusedwhenthepatientwasspeaking.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page5of28Validating:"Everyonefeelsthatwayfromtimetotime.
"EmotionalDischarge"Never!
Never!
Never!
"Complaint:"Mychildrenneverlistentome.
"Transitional/Elicitation:"IwantedtoremembertotellyouaboutlastSaturday.
"ProblemPresentation:"Myhusbandlosthisjob.
"BehavioralAnalysis/Educational:"Doyouthinkhewouldresponddifferentlyifyoutelephonedfirst"Reflective:"Youseemtobesayingthatyouwouldn'tlikethat.
"Irreverent"Wellhecertainlyfailedthattime!
"Table2.
showsthedistributionfrequencyofeachsym-bolinthealphabetforallthreesessions.
Thedistributioncomputedusingallsessionsisunremarkable.
Thethera-pist'scontributionsconsistprimarilyofacknowledgments,elicitationsandproblempresentations.
Thehighfrequencyofpatientcomplaintsandemotionaldischargesisconsis-tentwithadiagnosisofborderlinepersonalitydisorder.
Thesymbolfrequencydistributionwasalsocalculatedforeachsessionwithaviewtodeterminingiflongitudi-nalchangesinsymbolfrequenciescouldofferinsightsintothepatient-therapistinteraction.
Wedefineaconsistentchangeasoneinwhichthefrequencyofappearanceofasymboleitherincreasesordecreasesoverallthreesessions.
Inthecaseofthepatient,onlyonevariableshowedaconsistentpattern;thefrequencyofpatientacknowledgmentsdecreased.
ThedecreasefromSession1toSession2was,however,minimal.
Otherwise,theonlyconsistentpatternswereseenintherapistbeha-vior.
Thefrequencyofeducationalinterventionsdecreased,andthefrequencyofreflectiveinterventionsincreased.
Thefrequencyofvalidatinginterventionsfromthetherapistdecreasedoverthethreesessions.
Thispos-siblyreflectsthegrowingconfidencethatbothpartici-pantshadinthetherapeuticrelationship.
Asidefromdescribingpredictablechangesintherapistbehavior,theanalysisofsymbolfrequencieswaslargelyuninformative.
Thisissignificanttothepresentinvesti-gationbecauseitsuggeststheneedformeasuresthatquantifysequentialbehavior.
ResultsAnalysisofRepeatedPairsThemostelementaryformofsequentialanalysisistheanalysisofrepeatedpairs.
TheresultsfromthisanalysisTable2SymbolFrequencyDistributionContentSymbolFrequencyAllSessionsFirstSessionSecondSessionThirdSessionP:BehavioralAnalysis/Educationali.
1184.
1073.
1475.
1010P:Acknowledginga.
0988.
0978.
0947.
1024T:AcknowledgingA.
0899.
1041.
0638.
1038P:Information(Requesting/Providing)b.
0830.
0726.
0692.
0982P:Complaintf.
0798.
0915.
0692.
0827T:Transitional/ElicitationG.
0766.
0726.
0893.
0687T:ProblemPresentationH.
0735.
0536.
0820.
0757P:EmotionalDischargee.
0697.
0820.
0820.
0547T:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalI.
0659.
0820.
0729.
0533T:ValidatingD.
0532.
0915.
0474.
0407P:RequestforValidationc.
0532.
0599.
0455.
0561T:ReflectiveJ.
0450.
0221.
0492.
0519P:ProblemPresentationh.
0317.
0221.
0346.
0337T:Information(Requesting/Providing)B.
0298.
0221.
0328.
0309T:IrreverentK.
0108.
0032.
0000.
0224P:Transitional/Elicitationg.
0108.
0032.
0182.
0084P:Validatingd.
0051.
0126.
0018.
0042P:Irreverentk.
0044.
0000.
0000.
0098T:RequestforValidationC.
0006.
0000.
0000.
0014T:EmotionalDischargeE.
0000.
0000.
0000.
0000T:ComplaintF.
0000.
0000.
0000.
0000P:Reflectivej.
0000.
0000.
0000.
0000Thetableshowstherank-orderedfrequencyofeachsymbolinthesymbolicreduction.
Theexpectationfrequencyis.
0455.
TheletterTinthefirstcolumndenotestherapistcontributionsandtheletterPindicatespatientcontributions.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page6of28aftercombiningallthreetherapysessionsareshowninTable3.
Theexpectationfrequencyofarepeatedpairisp=.
0021.
Ninerepeatedpairsappearwithafrequencythatisatleastoneorderofmagnitudegreaterthantheexpectationfrequency.
Mostoftherepeatedpairsareassociatedwithwhatmightbedescribedasthemechanicsoftherapy:requesting,presentingandacknowledginginformation.
Asinthecaseofsinglesymbolfrequencies,patientcomplaintsandemotionaldischargesappearfrequentlyaselementsinrepeatedpairs.
AnalysisofRepeatedTriplesWhenrepeatedtriplesareexaminedamarkedpatternofchangeincontentisseenoverthethreesessions.
InamessageoflengthLMthereareLM-2triples.
Nonethelessthere,areveryfewrepeatedtriplesintheclinicaldata.
DuringVisitOneninetriplesappearmorethan1%ofthetime.
DuringVisitTwoonlytwotriplesappearinmorethan1%ofthesample,andinVisitThree,fourtriplesappearatafrequencyexceeding1%(Table4).
Thereisachangeinthecontentofrepeatedtriplesoverthethreesessions.
Inthefirstsessionthemostfre-quentlyobservedtripleisarequestforvalidationbythepatientfollowedbyanemotionaldischargefollowedbyacomplaint.
Thesethreecodingelementsappearpromi-nentlyintheotherrepeatedtriplesobservedduringthefirstsession.
Bythesecondsession,whichoccurredfourmonthsafterthefirstsession,behavioralanalysisbythetherapistandacknowledgmentofthesecommunicationsbythepatientarethemostfrequentlyobservedtriples.
Thispatternisconsistentwithclinicalexpectations.
Intheearlysessions,thepatient-therapistrelationshipisconstructedbythetherapist'snonjudgmentalacceptanceofthepatient'scomplaints,emotionaldischargesandneedforvalidation.
Thisisparticularlytrueinthecourseofborderlinepersonalitydisorder.
Theworkoftherapy,implementedbybehavioralanalysisandeducation,beginsaftertheconstructionofthetherapeuticalliance.
ContextFreeGrammarComplexityWhileseveralmethodscanbeusedtocharacterizeasymbolsequence,weconsiderfirstmeasuresofcom-plexity.
Quantitativemeasuresofcomplexitycanbemostreadilyintroducedbyconsideringanexplicitexample.
Considertwomessages,thatistwosymbolsequences,M1andM2.
M1=AAAAAAAABBBBBBBBCCCCCCCCDDDDDDDDM2=BCBADBCADBBDAAADAADDBCCCCDCACBBDItshouldbenotedthatbothmessageshavethesamesymbolfrequency,eightappearancesofeachsymbol.
Theyareindistinguishablewithdistribution-determinedmeasures,forexampleShannoninformation,butM2ismorecomplexthanM1inourconventionalunderstand-ingoftheterm.
Thereareseveralmethodsforquantify-ingthecomplexityofsymbolsequences.
Ataxonomyofcomplexitymeasureshasbeenpublished[73].
Inthefirstinstance,weconsiderthecontextfreegrammarcomplexityintroducedbyJiménez-Montao[60](adescriptionisgiveninAppendixOne).
Consistentwithourqualitativeexpectations,itisfoundthatthatgram-marcomplexityofM1is20bitsandthecomplexityofM2is27bits.
Thecomplexityofanobservedsymbolsequenceisoftenexpressedinbits/unittimebydividingthecom-plexityofthemessagebytheperiodofobservation[74].
TheresultsfromthethreetherapysessionsareshowninFigure1.
Complexitygenerationisseentoincreaseacrossthethreesessions.
(Theprocedureusedtoesti-matetheuncertaintiesofthesecomplexityvaluesisout-linedinAppendixOne).
Thisresultisconsistentwiththeincreaseinthenum-berofsymbolsgeneratedinthethreesessions(NDATA=317,549,713respectively).
WhileanyobservationbasedTable3HighFrequencyRepeatedPairsFirstElementofPairSecondElementofPairFrequencyT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:Acknowledging.
0279T:ProblemPresentationP:Acknowledging.
0260P:AcknowledgingT:Behavioralanalysis/Educational.
0247P:Information(Requesting/Providing)T:Acknowledging.
0241T:AcknowledgingP:Information(Requesting/Providing).
0234T:ProblemPresentationP:BehavioralAnalysis/Educational.
0234P:EmotionalDischargeP:Complaint.
0228T:Transitional/ElicitationP:BehavioralAnalysis/Educational.
0222P:ComplaintT:Acknowledging.
0209MostFrequentlyObservedRepeatedPairs.
Analyzedoverallthreesessions,thefrequenciesofninerepeatedpairsexceedtheexpectationfrequencyof.
0021byatleastoneorderofmagnitude.
Pdenotesthepatient,andTdenotesthetherapist.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page7of28onasinglecasehistorymustbestatedcircumspectly,theincreaseinthefrequencyofsubjecttransitionwhichisreflectedintheincreaseinNDATAoverthethreesessionsisconsistentwithqualitativeclinicalobservationswithborderlinepatients.
Aspatientsprogressintherapyonecan,insomeinstances,observeadecreasedperseverationintopicandagreaterflexibilityofdiscourse.
ThisresultisconsistentwiththequantitativeresultsofMcDaniel,etal.
[75]whofoundcorrelationsbetweenrateofimprovementandanestimateofthenumberofpatientutterances.
TheresultseenhereisalsoconsistentwiththeWinefield,etal.
[76]quantitativecharacterizationofapsychodynamicallyorientedpsychotherapywhichshoweddecreasingasymmetryinpatient/therapistverbalbehaviorduringthecourseoftreatment.
Thisdecreaseinasymmetrywasevidencedbyincreasedtherapistspeechactivity.
Increasedparticipationbythetherapistwouldresultinanincreaseinpatient-to-therapisttransitionsinthesymboltranscript,anincreaseinNDATA,andanincreaseincomplexitygeneration.
Itisalsoamatterofinteresttodeterminethestabilityofcomplexitywithinasession.
Thiswasdonebydeter-miningcomplexitygenerationforeachquartersession.
AvisualinspectionofFigure2suggeststhatthereisasomewhatgreaterwithin-sessionvariationinthethirdsession.
Thisisconsistentwithourunderstandingofanincreaseincomplexitygenerationduringthecourseofasuccessfultherapy.
Itisimportanttomakeadistinctionbetweenthecomplexityofamessageandtheintrinsicdynamicalcomplexityofthesystemthatgeneratedthemessage.
TheintrinsiccomplexityofthegeneratorcanbeTable4RepeatedTriplesAppearingataFrequencyExceeding1%VisitOneTripleFrequencyContentSymbol1ContentSymbol2ContentSymbol3cef.
022P:RequestValidationP:EmotionalDischargeP:ComplaintaIa.
019P:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingIaI.
016T:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalbfA.
016P:Information(Requesting/Providing)P:ComplaintT:Acknowledgingefc.
013P:EmotionalDischargeP:ComplaintP:RequestforValidationfAb.
013P:ComplaintT:AcknowledgingP:InformationHaI.
013T:ProblemPresentationP:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalbAb.
013P:Information(Requesting/Providing)T:AcknowledgingP:Information(Requesting/Providing)fce.
013P:ComplaintP:RequestValidationP:EmotionalDischargeVisitTwoTripleFrequencyContentSymbol1ContentSymbol2ContentSymbol3aIa.
015P:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingIai.
011T:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingP:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalVisitThreeTripleFrequencyContentSymbol1ContentSymbol2ContentSymbol3bAb.
018P:Information(Requesting/Providing)T:AcknowledgingP:Information(Requesting/Providing)IaI.
017T:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalAbA.
011T:AcknowledgingP:Information(Requesting/Providing)T:AcknowledgingaIa.
011P:AcknowledgingT:BehavioralAnalysis/EducationalP:AcknowledgingRepeatedTriplesAppearingataFrequencyExceeding1%.
Resultsarepresentedseparatelyforeachsession.
Pdenotesthepatient.
Tdenotesthetherapist.
123024681012GrammarComplexityforThreeTherapySessionsGrammarComplexity(bits/minute)TherapySessionFigure1Complexitygenerationinthreepsychotherapysessions.
Thecontextfreegrammarcomplexityofthesymbolicreductionofeachsessionwasnormalizedagainstthedurationofthesessiontodeterminecomplexitygenerationinbits/minute.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page8of28estimatedbycomparingthecomplexityofthemessageagainstthecomplexityofrandommessagesofequallengthgeneratedwiththesamesymbolset.
Theresultisthenormalizedcomplexity.
MathematicalproceduresforconstructingthisnormalizationareoutlinedinAppendixOne.
Thenormalizedcomplexityisdimen-sionlessandvariesbetweenavalueclosetozeroforaconstantsymbolsequence(onesymbolrepeatedthroughouttheentiremessage)andavalueofoneforarandomsequence.
Examplesgivingintermediatevaluesofnormalizedcomplexityareshownintheappendix.
Thenormalizedgrammarcomplexityofthethreetherapysessionsis.
765±.
033,.
758±.
015and.
763±.
017.
Thereisnosignificantchangeinthenor-malizedgrammarcomplexitywhichsuggeststhat,atleastinthistherapy,grammarcomplexitydidnotdetectchangesintheunderlyingdynamicalprocess.
ThecontrastbetweentheconsistencyofnormalizedcomplexityandtheincreaseincomplexityperunittimeisconsideredintheDiscussionsectionofthispaper.
Anexaminationofthenormalizedcomplexityforeachquarterofasessionallowsanexaminationofthestatio-narityoftheunderlyingdynamicalprocess(Figure3).
Theresultsaredisplayedon[0,1],thedefinedrangeofnormal-izedcomplexity.
Therearenosignificantwithin-sessionorbetween-sessiondifferenceswhenquartersessionsareanalyzed.
Acomparisonofthecomplexityvaluesobtainedwiththeoriginaltherapysymbolsequenceandcomplexityvaluesobtainedfromrandommessagesofthesamelengthmakesitpossibletoaddressthefollowingnullhypothesis:Asassessedbythiscomplexitymeasure,thesequen-tialstructureoftheoriginalmessageisindistinguish-ablefromthesequentialstructureofanequi-probable,randomsequenceofthesamelengthcon-structedfromthesamesymbolalphabet.
Severalstatisticaltestsofthenullhypothesishavebeenconsidered(AppendixOne).
WeuseheretheMonteCarloprobabilityofthenullhypothesis.
PNULL=NumberValues≤CORIG1+NSURRNSURRisthenumberofcomparisonrandommessages(calledsurrogates)computed.
Thenumberofcomplex-ityvaluestestedinthenumeratorincludesthecomplex-ityoftheoriginalsymbolsequenceaswellasthecomplexityvaluesobtainedwithsurrogates,ensuringthatthenumeratorhasavalueofatleastone.
Inthe123434567891011121314GrammarComplexityforThreeTherapySessionsGrammarComplexity(bits/minute)QuarterofSessionAnalyzedFigure2Withinsessioncomplexitygenerationforthreetherapysessions.
Grammarcomplexitygeneration(bits/minute)wasdeterminedseparatelyforeachquarterofeachsession.
ThetopcurvecorrespondstoSessionThree.
ThebottomcurvecorrespondstoSessionOne.
123400.
20.
40.
60.
81NormalizedGrammarComplexityforThreeTherapySessionsNormalizedGrammarComplexityQuarterofSessionAnalyzedFigure3Normalizedgrammarcomplexityforeachquarterofeachtherapysession.
Normalizedcomplexityisdefinedon[0,1].
ThegreenlinecorrespondstoSessionOne,thebluelinetoSessionTwoandtheredlinetoSessionThree.
Thecomplexityvaluesobtainedwithrandomnumbers(ablacklineatthetopofthegraph)andwithaconstantsymbolsequencewhereonesymbolisrepeatedthroughoutthemessage(ablacklineatthebottomofthegraph)areshownforcomparison.
Datasetsofthesamesizewereusedinthecomparisoncalculations.
Thenormalizedcomplexityobtainedwithrandomnumbersisapproximatelyone,andthenormalizedcomplexityobtainedwithaconstantsignalisapproximatelyzero.
DetailsofthecomparisoncalculationsaregiveninAppendixOne.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page9of28calculationsinFigures2and3,NSURR=499andCSurro-gate>CORIGinallcases.
ThenullhypothesisisrejectedwithPNULL=.
002;thatis,thesequentialstructureofpatient-therapistcommunicationinthesesessionsasscoredbythisprocedureandassessedbythismetricisnonrandom.
Lempel-ZivComplexityTheresultsobtainedwithgrammarcomplexitywereconfirmedbycalculationsofLempel-Zivcomplexity([77]describedinAppendixTwo).
Lempel-Zivcomplex-ityandgrammarcomplexityareinthesametaxonomicgroupofcomplexitymeasures(randomnessfinding,nonprobabilistic,modelbased).
ThevaluesobtainedwithLempel-Zivcomplexityarenotthesameasthoseobtainedwiththegrammarcomplexity,butthetwomeasuresshowthesamesensitivitytorandomnessinasymbolsequence.
TheLempel-ZivresultsanalogoustothoseobtainedwithgrammarcomplexityareshowninFigure4.
Asinthecaseofgrammarcomplexitythereisanincreaseincomplexitygenerationoverthethreesessions.
Thewithin-sessionvariabilityofLempel-Zivcomplex-ity(Figure5)showsthesamepatternthatwasobservedwithgrammarcomplexity.
Thewithin-sessionvariabilityisgreaterinSessionThree.
Lempel-Zivcomplexitycanalsobenormalizedbycomparisonswithrandomsurrogatesymbolstringspro-videdthatthecomplexityofthesurrogateisalsodeter-minedwiththeLempel-Zivalgorithm.
ThenormalizedLempel-Zivcomplexityforthethreesessionsis.
765±.
033,.
758±.
015and.
763±.
017respectively.
Incommonwithgrammarcomplexity,nochangeinthegeneratingdynamicalprocesswasdetectedwithLem-pel-Zivcomplexity.
Theseresultsshouldnotbegeneral-izedinappropriately.
Itremainspossiblethatsignificantchangemightbedetectedifadifferentmeasurewasappliedtothesamedata.
Itcanonlybesaidthatnor-malizedgrammarcomplexityandnormalizedLempel-Zivcomplexityfailedtodetectanybetween-sessionchangeswhilechangeswereseenincomplexitygenera-tionrateswithbothmeasures.
Aspreviouslynoted,thebetweensessionconsistencyofnormalizedcomplexityandtheincreaseincomplexityperunittimeisconsid-eredintheDiscussionsection.
Thewithin-sessionnor-malizedcomplexitywasalsocomputedwiththeLempel-Zivalgorithm(Figure6).
Asinthecaseofgrammarcomplexity,nosignificantwithin-sessionchangeswereseeninthenormalizedcomplexity.
Asbefore,thesurrogatenullhypothesisofrandomstructurewasrejectedbyLempel-ZivcomplexitywithPNULL=.
002(NSURR=499)inallcases.
Itcanagainbeconcludedthatpatient-therapistcommunicationhasnonrandomstructure.
MutualInformationConsidertwosimultaneouslyobservedsymbolsetsMes-sageA=(A1,A2,.
.
.
.
.
AN)andMessageB=(B1,B2,.
.
.
.
.
BN)constructedfromthesamealphabetofNaelements.
PA(I)istheprobabilityoftheappearanceofSymbolIinMessageAPB(J)istheprobabilityoftheappearanceofSymbolJinMessageB.
PAB(I,J)istheprobabilitythatSymbolIappearsinMessageAandSymbolJappearsin12300.
511.
522.
533.
544.
55LempelZivComplexityforThreeTherapySessionsLempelZivComplexity(bits/minute)TherapySessionFigure4Complexitygenerationinthreepsychotherapysessions.
TheLempel-Zivcomplexityofthesymbolicreductionofeachsessionwasnormalizedagainstthedurationofthesessiontodeterminecomplexitygenerationinbits/minute.
123422.
533.
544.
555.
566.
57LempelZivComplexityforThreeTherapySessionsLempelZivComplexity(bits/minute)QuarterofSessionAnalyzedFigure5Withinsessioncomplexitygenerationforthreetherapysessions.
Lempel-Zivcomplexitygeneration(bits/minute)wasdeterminedseparatelyforeachquarterofeachsession.
ThetopcurvecorrespondstoSessionThree.
ThebottomcurvecorrespondstoSessionOne.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page10of28MessageBatthesametime.
Theaveragemutualinfor-mationofMessagesAandBistheaveragenumberofbitsofMessageBthatcanbepredictedbymeasuringMessageA.
ItisdenotedbyI(A,B).
Itcanbeshown[78]thatI(A,B)=NαI=1NαJ=1PAB(I,J)log2PAB(I,J)PA(I)PB(J)MutualinformationissymmetricalI(A,B)=I(B,A).
Also,iftwoprocessesarestatisticallyindependentthenPAB(I,J)=PA(I)PB(J),andI(A,B)=0.
ThespecialcasewhereMessageAandMessageBarethesame,I(A,A),iscalledself-information.
Inthisapplication,weexaminetheabilityofames-sagetopredictitsownfuture.
WedefineI(AI,AI+1)asthemutualinformationobservedwhenMessageA=(A1,A2,.
.
.
.
.
AN-1)andMessageB=(A2,A3,.
.
.
.
.
AN).
Thiscanbegeneralizedtoconsiderlargertemporaldisplace-ments.
I(AI,AI+K)iscalculatedbysettingMessageA=(A1,A2,.
.
.
.
.
AN-K)andMessageB=(AK,AK+1,.
.
.
.
.
AN).
Thetimeshiftedself-informationisanonlinearmeasureoftemporaldecorrelation.
ExplanatoryexamplesaregiveninCellucci,etal[79].
IfamessagehasstrongtemporalpredictabilitythenI(AI,AI+K)remainshighasKisincreased.
Iftheprocessgeneratingamessageisdyna-micallydisordered,thenI(AI,AI+K)decreasesrapidlyasKincreases.
MutualinformationforthecaseK=1hasbeenappliedtotheexaminationofthesequence-sensitivestructureofnarrativecomponentsintheautobiographi-calspeechofanorexicadolescentsandcontrols[66].
TheseinvestigatorsfoundthatI(AI,AI+1)issignificantlylowerinpatients.
TheyalsocomparedI(AI,AI+1)valuesobtainedwiththeirdataagainstthevaluesobtainedwithrandomshufflesurrogatesandfoundthatsurrogatesdecorrelatefasterthantheoriginalsymbolsequenceindicatingthepresenceofnon-randomstructureintheoriginalsymbolsequence.
Figure7showsmutualinformationI(AI,AI+K)asafunctionofthetemporalshiftparameterKforthethreetherapysessions.
ThemutualinformationmeasuredinthefirstsessiondecorrelatesmoreslowlythanmutualinformationobtainedwithSessionTwoandThreeindi-catingahigherdegreeofpredictabilityinSessionOne.
ThisisconsistentwiththepreviousobservationofalowercomplexitygenerationrateinSessionOne.
Theseparationofmutualinformationfunctionsbetweenthefirstandsecondsessionandthefirstandthirdsessionissignificant(P0.
Hnquantifiestheaverageamountofinformationcontainedinasubstringoflengthn,andthereforeismonotoneincreasinginn.
Therelatedcon-ditionalentropies,hn,aregivenbyhn=Hn+1Hnhnistheaverageamountofinformationneededtopredictthenextsymbolinasubstringifthefirstnsym-bolsareknown,givinghn≥hn+1.
ThevaluesofHnandhnasafunctionofordernforthethreetherapysessionsareshowninFigure8.
Ateachorder,thevaluesofHnobtainedinthethirdses-sionaregreaterthanthevaluesobtainedinthesecondsessionwhicharegreaterthanthevaluesobtainedinthefirstsession.
Thisresultisconsistentwiththepre-viouslypresentedrateofcomplexitygeneration(Session3>Session2>Session1)andwiththeobservationthatmutualinformation,whichisrelatedtoentropy,decorr-elatesfasterinthelatersessions.
Thebetween-sessionseparationofconditionalentropyislessmarked,buttheconditionalentropyofSession3isgreaterthanthatofSession1atallordersofn.
Asinthecaseofmutualinformation,theresultsoftheseentropycalculationsmustbeviewedwithcare.
AsimpleanalysisindicatesthatlengtheffectswillcauseasignificantdeteriorationinanestimateofHnasnincreases,ifoneusestheequationforHngivenabove.
AmessageofNsymbolscontainsN-(n-1)n-words.
Aspreviouslynotedthenumberofpossiblen-wordsintheabsenceofrestrictiverulesis(Na)n.
Thusthenumberofpossiblen-wordsincreasesexponentiallywithordern,whilethenumberofwordsactuallypresentislimitedbyN.
Letμ(n)ibetheexpectationvalueofthenumberofappearancesofthei-thn-wordforthecaseofanequi-probabledistribution.
μ(n)i=p(n)iN=N/Nmax=N/(Nα)nThecalculationofHnusingthepreviousequationiswarrantedinthecaseofgoodstatisticswhichisobtainedwhenμ(n)iisontheorderoften[87].
InthepresentanalysisNa=22,andthesmallestvalueofNisobtainedinSession1whereN=317.
Thecriterionμ(n)i≥10issatisfiedforn=1whereHnforSession1,themeanvalueofcomplexitycalculatedfromallpossiblesubstringsoflengthLM/2,ands1/2thestandarddevia-tionofthatmean.
Expressedasafraction,uncertaintyiss1/2/,andΔCORIGisgivenbyCORIG=CORIGσ1/2Thisprocedureis,however,computationallyinsuppor-tableforlongermessages.
SupposeLM=8000.
Thispro-cedureforestimatingwouldrequireaveraging4000valuesofcomplexitycalculatedfromstringsoflengthLM/2=4000.
Wehaveadoptedtheprocedureofcalculatingfrom100stringsoflengthLM/2.
TheyareselectedrandomlyfromthesetofallpossibleLM/2substrings.
IncaseswhereLMiscal-culatedfromallpossiblesubstringsoflengthLM/2.
Aqualitativeunderstandingofthecomplexityofasymbolssequencecanbeobtainedbyapplyingthesemeasurestosymbolsequencesgeneratedbystandardsystemsthatarecommonlyexaminedindynamicalsys-temstheory.
Fiveexamplesareconsideredhere:acon-stantsequence(thesamesymbolisrepeated),sequencesgeneratedbytheRsslerandLorenzsystems(boththreedimensionalordinarydifferentialequations),theHénonsystem(atwodimensionaldifferenceequation)andarandomnumbergenerator.
Thetechnicalspecifi-cationsofthesystemsaregiveninAppendixThree.
TheRssler,Lorenz,Hénonandrandomdataareexpressedasrealvariables.
Inordertoapplyasymbolicdynamics-basedmeasureofcomplexity,itisnecessarytoprojectthesedatasetstoadiscretesymbolset.
Thereareseveralpossibleproceduresfordoingthis.
Radhak-rishnan,etal.
[165]usedK-meansclustering.
Whileconceptuallyattractive,theresultsofK-meansclusteringcanbeverysensitivetoinitialconditions.
BradleyandFayyad[166]addressedthissensitivitybyconstructingaK-meansalgorithmthatproducesarefinedinitialcondi-tionthatimprovedperformance.
Insofarasweknow,thismethodhasnotbeenappliedtotheproblemofconvertingrealdatatosymbolicdata.
AnalternativeapproachhasbeenpublishedbyHirata,etal.
[167]whoapproximateageneratingpartitionfromatimeseriesusingtessellations.
Thisisacomputationallydemandingprocedureandtherearepracticalissuesconcerningthesensitivityofthepartitionontheinitialization.
Steuer,etal[86]recommendusingthepartitionthatmaximizesRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page18of28entropy.
Inthepresentexamples,thecontinuousvari-abletimeseriesispartitionedaboutthemedian.
Inthisprocess,themedianiscomputedfromtheoriginaltimeseries.
Arealvariableisreplacedbysymbol'0'ifitislessthanthemedianandbysymbol'1'ifitisgreaterthanorequaltothemedian.
Thechoiceofthemedianratherthanthemeaniscriticaltothisprocess.
False-positiveindicationsofdeterministicstructureinrandomdatacanresultifthemeanisused[168].
Thepartition-ingprocessisdepictedinFigure9.
(Itshouldbenotedthatinthepresentpaper,theconsiderationofpartition-ingprotocolonlyappliestothedidacticexamplespre-sentedintheappendices.
Thepsychotherapydataaresymbolicandpartitioningisnotrequired).
ThegrammarcomplexityvaluescomputedfromonethousandelementsymbolsequencesgeneratedbythesemodelsystemsareshowninFigure10.
Theresultsareseentobeconsistentwithourqualitativeunderstandingofcomplexity.
Theconstantsequencegivesthelowestvalue,andtherandomnumbergeneratorproducesthelargestvalue.
TheorderingRsslerlessthanLorenz,lessthanHénonisalsoconsistentwithexpectationsbasedonavisualexaminationofthetimeseriesintheleftcol-umnofFigure9.
Acriticaldistinctionmustbemadebetweenthecom-plexityofamessage,CORIGandtheintrinsiccomplexityoftheprocessthatgeneratedthemessage.
Thevalueofgrammarcomplexitywilldependontwofactors,thecomplexityofthedynamicalprocessgeneratingthesymbolsequenceandthelengthofthesymbolsequence.
ThisisseenintheupperpanelofFigure11wheregrammarcomplexityisplottedasafunctionofthelengthofthedataset.
Theorderingofcomplexityvaluesseenwith1000elementsequencesinFigure2ispreserved(random>Hénon>Lorenz>Rssler>con-stant)andthevaluesincreasewiththesizeofthedataset.
Itisthereforenecessarytofindaneffectivenormal-izationofcomplexityvaluesthatallowscomparisonofintrinsiccomplexitieswithoutthecomplicationofdatasetsize.
ItmightbesupposedthatdividingCORIGbythelengthofthemessageisanacceptablesolution.
Ithasbeenshownthatthisisnotthecase[169].
AneffectivenormalizationofCORIGcanbeachievedbycomparingitagainstthevaluesofcomplexityobtainedfromrandomequiprobablemessagesofthesamelength.
LetNabethesizeofthesymbolalphabet(thenumberofdistinctsymbolsavailableformessageconstruction,intheseexamplesNa=2).
NaisnotmessagelengthLM.
Anequiprobablesurrogateisonewhereeachsymbolappearswithprobability1/Na.
Letdenotetheaveragevalueofcomplexityobtainedfromrandomequi-probablesurrogatesoflengthLM(thesubscriptsdenotesasurrogate).
ThenormalizedcomplexityisdefinedbyCN=CORIG/CNrangesfromclosetozeroformessagescontainingasinglerepeatedsymboltoclosetooneformessagesgeneratedbyrandomprocesses.
AsoutlinedinRapp[170],CNcannotbeformedbynormalizingagainstrandomshufflesurrogates.
ConsiderthecaseofamessagethatconsistsofasinglerepeatedsymbolselectedfromanalphabetofsizeNa>1.
(SequencesinamessagespaceofNa=1consistofasinglesymbolandonlydifferbylength.
Trivially,theircomplexityisthenumberofbitsrequiredtoencodelengthLM.
)AneffectivenormalizationshouldgivealowvalueofCNtoarepeatedsymbolmessage.
Supposesur-rogateswereformedbyarandomshuffle.
Sincethemessagecontainsonlyonesymbol,theyallhavethesamevalueofcomplexity.
Inthiscase,CORIG=andhenceCN=1,whichisthecomplexityofarandommessage.
IfinsteadsurrogatesareequiprobableonNa,Na>1,thenisgreaterthanCORIGandCNhasalowvalue.
Alowvalueofcomplexityisexpectedforaconstantsequence.
TheuncertaintyinCN,ΔCN,canbeestimatedbythefollowingargumentCN=CORIG/(CN)2=CNCORIG2(CORIG)2+CN2()2(CN)2=12(CORIG)2+CORIG22()2TheestimationofΔCORIGhasbeendiscussed.
isthemeancomplexitycomputedfromadistributionofequiprobablesurrogates.
Δisthestandarddevia-tionofthatdistribution.
ComparisonofCORIGandthecomplexityvaluesobtainedwithsurrogatesmakesitpossibletoaddressthefollowingsurrogatenullhypothesis:Asassessedbythiscomplexitymeasure,thesequen-tialstructureoftheoriginalmessageisindistinguish-ablefromthesequentialstructureofanequi-probable,randomsequenceofthesamelengthcon-structedfromthesamesymbolalphabet.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page19of280501001502002501050510ContinuousVariableRossler05010015020025000.
51BinaryPartition050100150200250201001020Lorenz05010015020025000.
5105010015020025010.
500.
51Henon05010015020025000.
5105010015020025000.
51IndexRandom05010015020025000.
51IndexFigure9Partitioningrealdataontoadiscretesymbolset.
Themedianisdeterminedformtheoriginaldata.
Inthisexample,realvariablesarereplacedbythesymbol'0'iftheyarelessthanthemedianandbysymbol'1'iftheyaregreaterthanorequaltothemedian.
Thelocationsofmediansareindicatedbythehorizontalredlines.
Graphsintheleftcolumnshowtherealvariabletimeseries.
Thecorrespondingsymbolsequencesareshownintherightcolumn.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page20of28Severalstatisticaltestsofthenullhypothesishavebeenconsidered[168].
WeuseheretheMonteCarloprobabilityofthenullhypothesis.
PNULL=NumberValues≤CORIG1+NSURRNSURRisthenumberofsurrogatescomputed.
Thenumberofcomplexityvaluestestedinthenumeratorincludesthecomplexityoftheoriginalsymbolsequenceaswellasthecomplexityvaluesobtainedwithsurro-gates,ensuringthatthenumeratorhasavalueofatleastone.
Thisstatisticaltestwaschosenbecauseitisadistribution-agnostictest,thatis,itmakesnoassump-tionsaboutthestructureoftheCSurrogatedistribution.
Surrogateshavearandomstructure,andthegrammarcomplexitygivesthehighestvaluetorandomsequences.
WethereforeexpectthevaluesofCSurrogatetobegreaterthanthevalueofCORIGifanonrandomstructureispre-sentintheoriginalsequence.
ThesmallestvalueofPNULLwillbeobtainedwhenallvaluesofCSurrogatearegreaterthanCORIG.
Thatminimumvalueistherefore1/(1+NSURR).
InthecalculationsinFigures10and11,NSURR=499andCSurrogate>CORIGinallcasesfortheconstantsequence,Rssler,LorenzandHénondata.
ThenullhypothesisisthereforerejectedwithPNULL=.
002.
Asexpected,thenullhypothesisisnotrejectedbysymbolsequencesproducedbyarandomnumbergen-erator.
ForthetencasesinFigure11correspondingtoLM=1000,2000,.
.
.
10,000,theaveragevalueofPNULLobtainedwithrandomdataisPNULL=.
562Barnard[171]andHope[172]haveproposedanon-parametrictestforrejectingthenullhypothesis.
UndertheircriterionthenullhypothesisisrejectedifCORIGHénon>Lorenz>Rssler>constant(Figure12).
AsshowninFigure13,Lempel-Zivcomplexityshowsthesamedependenceonmessagelengththatwasobservedwithgrammarcomplexity.
Thenormalizationwithequi-probablerandomsurrogateswasalsoimple-mentedwithLempel-Zivcomplexity.
Asbefore,thenor-malizedcomplexityisindependentofLM.
Inthesecalculations,499surrogateswerecomputedandthenullhypothesiswasrejectedwithprobabilityPNIULL=.
002inallcaseswiththeexceptionofrandomdatawheretheaveragevalueofPNIULLwas.
450.
Aspreviouslyreported[169]grammarcomplexityandLempel-Zivcomplexityarehighlycorrelated.
Thecom-plexityvaluescomputedwithRssler,Lorenz,HénonandrandomdataforLM=1000,2000,.
.
.
.
10,000ele-mentdatasetswerecompared.
ThePearsonlinearcor-relationcoefficientwasfoundtober=.
998(thesamevaluethatwasobtainedwithdifferentdatain[169]).
12345020406080100120LempelZivComplexityforTestSystemsLempelZivComplexity(bits)ConstantRosslerLorenzHenonRandomFigure12Lempel-Zivcomplexityforonethousandsymbolsequencesgeneratedbymodelsystems.
Symbolsequencesweregeneratedbyabinarypartitionaboutthemedian.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page23of28Theprobabilityofthenullhypothesisofnocorrelationwaslessthan10-8.
AppendixThree:SpecificationofModelSystemFivemodelsystemsareinvestigatedinthecalculationspresentedinAppendicesOneandTwo.
Theconstantsymbolsequenceisconstructedbyrepeatingonesymbolfortheentirelengthofthedataset.
TheRsslersystem[173]isathreedimensionalsystemofautonomousordinarydifferentialequations.
dx/dt=yzdy/dt=x+aydz/dt=b+xzcza=.
2b=.
4c=5.
7Thedifferentialequationswereintegratedwithasteplengthofh=.
1usingasixthorderRunge-Kutta-Huttaalgorithm[174].
TheLorenzsystem[175,176]isspeci-fiedbydx/dt=σ(xy)dy/dt=xz+rxydz/dt=xybzσ=10b=8/3r=28AsinthecaseoftheRsslerequations,aRunge-Kutta-Huttacalculationwasperformedwithh=.
1.
TheHénonsystem[177,178]isatwodimensionaldifferenceequation.
xt+1=1ax2t+ytyt+1=bxta=1.
4b=0.
30200040006000800010000120000200400600800LempelZivcomplexityandnormalizedcomplexityasafunctionofdatasetsizeLZComplexity(bits)RandomHenonLorenzRosslerConstant02000400060008000100001200000.
51NormalizedComplexityDataSetSizeRandomHenonLorenzConstantRosslerFigure13Lempel-Zivcomplexityandnormalizedcomplexityasafunctionofdatasetsize.
Symbolsequencesweregeneratedfromthepreviouslydefinedmodelsystemsusingabinarypartitionaboutthemedian.
TheupperpanelshowsLempel-Zivcomplexity.
ThelowerpanelshowsthecorrespondingnormalizedcomplexitywherevaluesofLempel-Zivcomplexityarenormalizedagainstrandom,equi-probablesurrogates.
Inthesecalculations499surrogateswereusedtocomputeCN(modifiedfromRapp,2007).
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page24of28Therandomnumbergenerator[179]produceduni-formlydistributedrandomnumberson[0,1].
ItisbasedonL'Ecuyer'stwo-sequencegenerator[180]andincor-poratesaBays-Durhamshuffle[181].
AcknowledgementsPERwouldliketoacknowledgetrainingreceivedatthePhiladelphiaSchoolofPsychoanalysisandthePhiladelphiaConsultationCenterandspecificallyDr.
StephenDayEllis,President,andDr.
AngelaSandone-Barr,DirectorofClinicalServices.
DiscussionswithDr.
ArnoldFeldmanandwiththemembersoftheEntwurfGruppeareacknowledgedwithgratitude.
TheopinionsandassertionscontainedhereinaretheprivateopinionsoftheauthorsandarenottobeconstruedasofficialorreflectingtheviewsoftheUnitedStatesDepartmentofDefense.
MAJMacknowledgespartialsupportfrom:SistemaNacionaldeInvestigadores;PROMEP,Project:UV-CA-197MEXICO,andUniversidadVeracruzana.
PERwouldliketoacknowledgesupportfromtheTraumaticInjuryResearchProgramoftheUniformedServicesUniversityoftheHealthSciencesandfromtheDefenseMedicalResearchandDevelopmentProgram.
Authordetails1DepartmentofMilitaryandEmergencyMedicine,UniformedServicesUniversity,4301JonesBridgeRoad,Bethesda,MD20814,USA.
2Aquinas,LLC,2014St.
AndrewsDrive,Berwyn,PA19312,USA.
3HuntonandWilliamsLLP,2200PennsylvaniaAve.
NW,Washington,DC20037,USA.
4DepartmentofEpidemiologyandPublicHealth,UniversityofMarylandSchoolofMedicine,HowardHall,Suite200,660W.
RedwoodStreet,Baltimore,MD20201USA.
5FacultaddeFísicaeInteligenciaArtificial,UniversidadVeracruzana,SebastiánCamacho#5,ColCentro,Xalapa,Ver.
91000,Mexico.
6DepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofWashington,Box355915,Seattle,WA,98195,USA.
Authors'contributionsPERwrotethesoftwaretocomputeLempel-Zivcomplexity,softwaretoconfirmcalculationsofcontextfreecomplexity,performedthecomplexitycalculationsandwastheprimaryauthorofthemanuscript.
AMKGsupervisedthestatisticalanalysisoftheresults.
MAJMwrotethesoftwareforgrammarcomplexitycalculationsandcomputedtherepeatedpairsanalysis.
CJCcontributedcalculationsofmutualinformation,n-thorderentropiesandconditionalentropies.
KEKwasthetreatingpsychotherapistandledtheprocessofrestatingthetherapyprotocolsassymbolsequences.
Allauthorshavereadandapprovedthefinalmanuscript.
CompetinginterestsTheauthorsdeclarethattheyhavenocompetinginterests.
Received:27July2010Accepted:27July2011Published:27July2011References1.
HibbardMR,UysalS,KeplerK,BogdanyJ,SilverJ:AxisI.
psychopathologyinindividualswithtraumaticbraininjury.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1998,13:24-39.
2.
HibbardMR,GordonWA,OtheraLM:Traumaticbraininjury.
InCognitive-BehavioralStrategiesinCrisisIntervention.
Editedby:DattiloFM,FreedmanA.
NewYork:GuilfordPress;2000:219-242.
3.
NationalInstitutesofHealth:Rehabilitationofpersonswithtraumaticbraininjury.
NIHConsensusStatement1998,16(1):26-28,October.
4.
ZaloshnjaE,MillerT,LangloisJ,SelassieA:Prevalenceoflong-termdisabilityfromtraumaticbraininjuryinthecivilianpopulationoftheUnitedStates,2005.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation2008,23(6):394-400.
5.
FannJR,KatonWJ,UomotoJM,EsselmanPC:Psychiatricdisordersandfunctionaldisabilityinout-patientswithtraumaticbraininjuries.
AmericanJournalofPsychiatry1995,152(10):1493-1499.
6.
VanReekumR,BolagoI,FinlaysonMS,GarnerA,LinksPS:Psychiatricdisordersaftertraumaticbraininjury.
BrainInjury1996,10:319-327.
7.
AshmanTA,SpielmanSA,HibbardMR,SilverJM,ChandnaT,GordonWA:Psychiatricchallengesinthefirst6yearsaftertraumaticbraininjury:cross-sequentialanalysesofAxisIdisorders.
ArchivesofPhysicalMedicineandRehabilitation2004,85(Supplement2):S36-S42.
8.
DebS,LyonsI,KoutzouskisC,AliK,McCarthyG:Rateofpsychiatricillness1yearaftertraumaticbraininjury.
AmericanJournalofPsychiatry1999,156:374-378.
9.
RosenthalM,ChristensenBK,RossTP:Depressionfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
ArchivesofPhysicalMedicineandRehabilitation1998,79:90-103.
10.
IversonGL:Misdiagnosisofpersistentpostconcussionsyndromeinpatientswithdepression.
ArchivesofClinicalNeuropsychology2006,21:303-310.
11.
CopeDN:Anintegrationofpsychopharmacologicalandrehabilitationapproachestotraumaticbraininjuryrehabilitation.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1994,9:1-18.
12.
PrigatanoGP:Disorderedmind,woundedsoul:theemergingroleofpsychotherapyinrehabilitationafterbraininjury.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1991,6(4):1-10.
13.
Ben-YishayY,RattokJ,LakinP,PiasetskyEB,RossB,SilverS,ZideE,EzrachiO:Neuropsychologicrehabilitation:Questforaholisticapproach.
SeminarsinNeurology1985,5(3):252-258.
14.
EzrachiO,Ben-YishayY,KayT,DiUerL,RattokJ:Predictingemploymentintraumaticbraininjuryfollowingneuropsychologicalrehabilitation.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1991,6(3):71-84.
15.
TeriL,LogsdonRG,UomotoJ,McCurrySM:Behavioraltreatmentofdepressionindementiapatients:acontrolledclinicaltrial.
JournalofGerontologyB.
PsychologicalSciences1997,52B:159-166.
16.
GoransonTE,GraveRE,AllisonD,LaFrenieresR:Communityintegrationfollowingmultidisciplinaryrehabilitationfortraumaticbraininjury.
BrainInjury2003,17:759-774.
17.
WilliamsWH,EvansJJ,WilsonBA:Neurorehabilitatonfortwocasesofpost-traumaticstressdisorderfollowingbraininjury.
CognitiveNeuropsychiatry2003,8:1-18.
18.
TierskyLA,AnselmiV,JohnstonMV,KurtykaJ,RosenE,SchwartzT,DeLucaJ:Atrialofneuropsychologicrehabilitationinmild-spectrumtraumaticbraininjury.
ArchivesofPhysicalMedicineandRehabilitation2005,86(8):1565-1574.
19.
BeutlerLE,MoleiroC,TalebiH:Howpractitionerscansystematicallyuseempiricalevidenceintreatmentselection.
JournalofClinicalPsychology2002,58:1199-1212.
20.
PayneHC:Traumaticbraininjury,depressionandcannabisuse-assessingtheireffectsoncognitiveperformance.
BrainInjury2000,15:479-489.
21.
MeddJ,TateRL:Evaluationofanangermanagementprogrammefollowingacquiredbraininjury:apreliminarystudy.
NeuropsychologicalRehabilitation2000,10(2):185-201.
22.
SohlbergMM:Psychotherapyapproaches.
InNeuropsychologicalManagementofMildTraumaticBrainInjury.
Editedby:RaskinSA,MateerCA.
NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress;2000:137-156.
23.
CiceroneK:Psychotherapeuticinterventionswithtraumaticallybrain-injuredpatients.
RehabilitationPsychology1989,34:105-114.
24.
RuffRM,CamenzuliL,MuellerJ:Miserableminority:emotionalriskfactorsthatinfluencetheoutcomeofamildtraumaticbraininjury.
BrainInjury1996,10(8):551-566.
25.
AnsonK,PonsfordJ:Evaluationofacopingskillsgroupfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
BrainInjury2006,20:167-178.
26.
MateerCA,SiraCS:CognitiveandemotionalconsequencesofTBI:interventionstrategiesforvocationalrehabilitation.
Neurorehabilitation2006,21:315-326.
27.
LewisL,RosenbergSJ:Psychoanalyticpsychotherapywithbrain-injuredadultpsychiatricpatients.
JournalofNervousandMentalDisease1990,178(2):69-77.
28.
GoldsteinK:Theeffectsofbraindamageonthepersonality.
Psychiatry1952,15:245-260.
29.
FolzerSM:Psychotherapywith"mild"brain-injuredpatients.
AmericanJournalofOrthopsychiatry2001,71(2):245-251.
30.
WermanDS:ThePracticeofSupportivePsychotherapyNewYork:Brunner/Mazel;1988.
31.
PrigatanoGP,KlonoffPS:Psychotherapyandneuropsychologicalassessmentafterbraininjury.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1988,3:45-68.
32.
DelmonicoRL,Hanley-PetersonP,EnglanderJC:Grouppsychotherapyforpersonswithtraumaticbraininjury:Managementoffrustrationandsubstanceabuse.
JournalofHeadTraumaRehabilitation1998,13(6):10-22.
33.
RushAJ,FirstMB,BlackerD:HandbookofPsychiatricMeasures.
Secondedition.
Washington,DC:AmericanPsychiatricPublishers;2008.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page25of2834.
BlanchetA,BattM,TrognonA,MasseL:Languageandbehaviourpatternsinatherapeuticinteractionsequence.
InTheHiddenStructureofInteraction.
Editedby:AnolliLDuncanS,MagnussonMSRivaG.
Amsterdam:IOSPress;2005:123-139,Amsterdam:IOSPress.
35.
MagnussonMS:Understandingsocialinteraction:discoveringhiddenstructurewithmodelandalgorithms.
InTheHiddenStructureofInteraction.
Editedby:AnolliLDuncanS,MagnussonMSRivaG.
Amsterdam:IOSPress;2005:,Amsterdam:IOSPress.
36.
Haynal-ReymondV,JonssonGK,MagnussionMS:Nonverbalcommunicationindoctor-suicidalpatientinterview.
InTheHiddenStructureofInteraction.
Editedby:AnolliLDuncanS,MagnussonMSRivaG.
Amsterdam:IOSPress;2005:141-148,Amsterdam:IOSPress.
37.
MertenJ,SchwabF:Facialexpressionpatternsincommonpsychotherapeuticsituations.
InTheHiddenStructureofInteraction.
Editedby:AnolliLDuncanS,MagnussonMSRivaG.
Amsterdam:IOSPress;2005:149-158,Amsterdam:IOSPress.
38.
MagnussonMS:Hiddenreal-timepatternsinintra-andinter-individualbehavior:descriptionanddetection.
EuropeanJournalofPsychologicalAssessment1996,12:112-123.
39.
MagnussonMS:Discoveringhiddentimepatternsinbehavior:T-patternsandtheirdetection.
BehaviorResearchMethods,InstrumentsandComputers2000,32(1):93-110.
40.
RappPE,Jiménez-MontaoMA,LangsRJ,ThomsonL:Quantitativecharacterizationofpatient-therapistcommunication.
MathematicalBiosciences1991,105:207-227.
41.
GrünzigHJ,HolzscheckK,KacheleH:ProgrammsystemzurmaschinellenInhaltsanalysevonPsychotherapieprotokollen.
MedizinischePsychologie1976,2:208-217.
42.
PennebakerJW,FrancisME,BoothRJ:LinguisticInquiryandWordCount(LIWC):LIWC2001Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum;2001.
43.
PennebakerJ,MehlM,NiederhofferK:Psychologicalaspectsofnaturallanguageuse:Ourwords,ourselves.
AnnualReviewofPsychology2003,54:547-577.
44.
HartRP:RedevelopingDICTION:theoreticalconsiderations.
InTheory,MethodandPracticeinComputerAnalysis.
Editedby:WestMD.
NewYork:Ablex;2001:43-60.
45.
FastLA,FunderDC:Personalityasmanifestinworduse:correlationswithself-report,acquaintancereport,andbehavior.
JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology2008,94:334-346.
46.
HoltRR:Bookreview:InSearchofPsychoanalyticTruthfromImpressiontoInquiry:ATributetotheWorkofRobertWallerstein.
InJournaloftheAmericanPsychoanalyticAssociation.
Volume56.
Editedby:WilmaBucci,NorbertFreedman.
London:InternationalPsychoanalyticAssociation;2008:(3):997-1009.
47.
DahlH:Aquantitativestudyofapsychoanalysis.
InPsychoanalysisandContemporaryScience.
Editedby:HoltRR,PeterfreundE.
NewYork:Macmillan;1972:237-257.
48.
DahlH:Themeasurementofmeaninginpsychoanalysisbycomputeranalysisofverbalcontexts.
JournaloftheAmericanPsychoanalyticAssociation1974,22:37-57.
49.
DahlH,TellerV,MossD,TrujilloM:Countertransferenceexamplesofthesyntacticexpressionofwardedoffcontents.
PsychoanalyticQuarterly1978,47:339-363.
50.
ChomskyN:ReflectionsonLanguageNewYork:Pantheon;1975.
51.
ChomskyN:Conditionsonrulesofgrammar.
LinguisticAnalysis1976,2(4):303-351.
52.
GottschalkLA,GleserGC:TheMeasurementofPsychologicalStatesThroughtheContentofVerbalBehaviorUniversityofCaliforniaPress,Berkeley,CA;1969.
53.
GottschalkLA,FoxRA,BatesDE:Astudyofpredictionandoutcomeinamentalhealthcrisisclinic.
AmericanJournalofPsychiatry1973,130:1107-1111.
54.
GottschalkLA,KunkelRL,WohlT,SaengerE,WingetCN:Totalandhalfbodyirradiation:effectoncognitiveandemotionalprocesses.
ArchivesofGeneralPsychiatry1969,21:574-580.
55.
PerleyJ,WingetCN,PlacciC:Hopeanddiscomfortasfactorsinfluencingtreatmentcontinuance.
ComprehensivePsychiatry1971,12:557-563.
56.
LebovitsAH,HollandJC:UseoftheGottschalk-Gleserverbalcontentanalysisscaleswithmedicallyillpatients.
PsychosomaticMedicine1983,45:305-320.
57.
GottschalkLA,Hoigaard-MartinJ:Emotionalimpactofmastectomy.
PsychiatryResearch1986,17:153-167.
58.
VineyLL,WestbrookMT:Psychologicalstatesinpatientswithdiabetesmellitus.
InContentAnalysisofVerbalBehavior.
Editedby:L.
A.
GottschalkLA,LolasF,VineyLL.
Berlin:SpringerVerlag;1986:157-169.
59.
LangsR,RappPE,PintoA,CramerG,BadalamentiA:Threequantitativestudiesofgenderandidentityinpsychotherapyconsultations.
AmericanJournalofPsychotherapy1992,46:183-206.
60.
Jiménez-MontaoMA:Onthesyntacticstructureofproteinsequencesandtheconceptofcomplexity.
BulletinofMathematicalBiology1984,46:641-659.
61.
LangsRJ:APrimerofPsychotherapyNewYork:Gardner;1988.
62.
StilesWB:DescribingTalk:ATaxonomyofVerbalResponseModesNewbury,CA:SagePublications;1992.
63.
StilesWB,SultanFE:Verbalresponsemodeusedbyclientsinpsychotherapy.
JournalofConsultingandClinicalPsychology1979,47(3):611-613.
64.
StilesWB,McDanielSH,McGaugheyK:Verbalresponsemodecorrelatesofexperiencing.
JournalofConsultingandClinicalPsychology1979,47(4):795-797.
65.
DobaK,PezardL,LesneA,VignauJ,ChristopheV,NandrinoJL:Dynamicsofemotionalexpressioninautobiographicspeechofpatientswithanorexianervosa.
PsychologicalReports2007,101:237-249.
66.
DobaK,NandrinoJL,LesneA,HumezC,PezardL:Organizationofnarrativecomponentsinautobiographicalspeechofanorexicadolescents:Astatisticalandnonlineardynamicalanalysis.
NewIdeasinPsychology2008,26:295-308.
67.
LeroyF,PezardL,NandrinoJL,BeauneD:Dynamicalquantificationofschizophrenicspeech.
PsychiatryResearch2005,133(2-3):159-171.
68.
KintschW,vanDijkTA:Towardamodeloftextcomprehensionandproduction.
PsychologicalReview1978,85(5):363-394.
69.
VanDijkTA,KintschW:StrategiesofDiscourseComprehensionNewYork:AcademicPress;1983.
70.
LinehanM:CognitiveandBehavioralTreatmentofBorderlinePersonalityDisorderTheGuildfordPress,NY;1993.
71.
LinehanMM:SkillsTrainingManualforTreatingBorderlinePersonalityDisorderNewYork:GuilfordPress;1993.
72.
StilesWB,ShapiroDA,Firth-CozensJA:Therapistdifferencesintheuseofverbalresponsemodeformsandintents.
Psychotherapy1989,26:314-322.
73.
RappPE,SchmahTI:Dynamicalanalysisinclinicalpractice.
InChaosinBrain.
Editedby:LehnertzK,ArnholdJGrassbergerP,ElgerCD.
WorldScientific,Singapore;2000:52-65.
74.
RappPE,ZimmermanID,ViningEP,CohenN,AlbanoAM,Jiménez-MontaoMA:Thealgorithmiccomplexityofneuralspiketrainsincreasesduringfocalseizures.
JournalofNeuroscience1994,14:4731-4739.
75.
McDanielSH,StilesWB,McGaugheyKJ:Correlationsofmalecollegestudent'sverbalresponsemodeuseinpsychotherapywithmeasuresofpsychologicaldisturbanceandpsychotherapyoutcome.
JournalofConsultingandClinicalPsychology1981,49(4):571-582.
76.
WinefieldHR,BassetDL,ChandlerMA,ProskeI:Processinpsychotherapyasdecreasingasymmetrybetweenpatientandtherapist:evidencefromverbalinteraction.
AmericanJournalofPsychotherapy1987,41(1):117-126.
77.
LempelA,ZivJ:Onthecomplexityoffinitesequences.
IEEETransactionsonInformationTheory1976,IT-22:75-81.
78.
GallagerRG:InformationTheoryandReliableCommunicationNewYork:JohnWileyandSons;1968.
79.
CellucciCJ,AlbanoAM,RappPE:Statisticalvalidationofmutualinformationcalculations:comparisonsofalternativenumericalalgorithms.
PhysicalReviewE2005,71,066208-1to066208-14.
80.
ShannonCE:Amathematicaltheoryofcommunication.
BellSystemTechnicalJournal1948,27:379-423.
81.
ShannonCE:PredictionandentropyofprintedEnglish.
BellSystemTechnicalJournal1951,30:50-64.
82.
BurtonNG,LickliderJCR:Long-rangeconstraintsinthestatisticalstructureofprintedEnglish.
AmericanJournalofPsychology1955,68(4):650-653.
83.
CoverT,KingR:AconvergentgamblingestimateoftheentropyofEnglish.
IEEETransactionsonInformationTheory1978,24(4):413-421.
84.
KolmogorovAN:Threeapproachestothequantitativedefinitionofinformation.
ProblemsofInformationProcessing1965,1:3-11,TranslatedfromtheRussianEdition.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page26of2885.
EbelingW:PredictionandentropyofnonlineardynamicalsystemsandsymbolicsequenceswithLRO.
PhysicaD1997,109(1-2):42-52.
86.
SteuerR,MolgedeyL,EbelingW,Jiménez-MontaoM-A:Entropyandoptimalpartitionfordataanalysis.
EuropeanPhysicalJournalSeriesB2001,19:265-269.
87.
SchmittAO,HerzelH,EbelingW:Anewmethodtocalculatehigher-orderentropiesfromfinitesamples.
EurophysicsLetters23:303-309.
88.
GrassbergerP:Towardaquantitativetheoryofself-generatedcomplexity.
InternationalJournalofTheoreticalPhysics1986,25(9):907-938.
89.
EbelingW,MuschikW,eds:StatisticalPhysicsandThermodynamicsofNonlinearNonequilbriumSystemsSingapore:WorldScientific;1992.
90.
GramssT:Entropyofthesymbolicsequenceforcriticalcirclemaps.
PhysicalReviewE1994,50(4):2616-2620.
91.
EbelingW,FeistelR,HerzelH:Dynamicsandcomplexitiesofbiomolecules.
PhysicaScripta1987,35:761-768.
92.
HerzelH:Complexityofsymbolsequences.
SystemsAnalysis,Modeling,Simulation1988,5(5):435-444.
93.
HerzelH,SchmittAO,EbelingW:Finitesampleeffectsinsequenceanalysis.
Chaos,SolitonsandFractals1994,4(1):97-113.
94.
SchmittA:StructuralanalysisofDNAsequences.
Ph.
D.
thesisHumboldt-Universitt,Berlin,Mathematisch-NaturwissenschaftlicheFakulttI;1995.
95.
SchürmannT,GrassbergerP:Entropyestimationofsymbolsequences.
Chaos1996,6:414-427.
96.
DudokdeWitT:WhendofinitesamplesizessignificantlyaffectentropyestimatesEuropeanPhysicalJournalB1999,11:513-516.
97.
LesneA,BlancJL,PezardL:Entropyestimationofveryshortsymbolsequences.
PhysicalReviewE2009,79:046208.
98.
Alvarez-LacalleE,DorowB,EckmannJP,MosesE:Hierarchicalstructuresinduceslong-rangedynamicalcorrelationsinwrittentexts.
ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciences,USA2006,103:7956-7961.
99.
ManningCD,SchützeH:FoundationsofStatisticalNaturallanguageProcessingCambridge,MA:MITPress;1999.
100.
BeefermanD,BergerA,LaffertyJD:Statisticalmodelsfortextsegmentation.
MachineLearning1999,34:177-210.
101.
NowakMA,KomarovaN,NiyogiP:Computationalandevolutionaryaspectsoflanguage.
Nature2002,417:611-617.
102.
MontemurroMA,ZanetteDH:Universalentropyofwordorderingacrosslinguisticfamilies.
PLoSOne2001,6(5):e19875.
103.
XuJ,LiuZR,LiuR:ThemeasureofsequencecomplexityforEEGstudies.
Chaos,SolitonsandFractals1994,4:2111-2119.
104.
RedingtonDJ,ReidbordSP:Chaoticdynamicsinautonomicnervous-systemactivityofapatientduringapsychotherapysession.
BiologicalPsychiatry1992,31:993-1007.
105.
BützMR:Practicalapplicationsfromchaostheorytothepsychotherapeuticprocess.
PsychologicalReports1993,73:543-554.
106.
SchiepekG,KowalikZJ,SchützA,KhlerM,RichterK,StrunkG,MühlnickelW,ElbertT:Psychotherapyasachaoticprocess.
I.
Codingtheclient-therapistinteractionbymeansofsequentialplananalysisandthesearchforchaos:astationaryapproach.
PsychotherapyResearch1997,7(2):173-194.
107.
KowalikZJ,SchiepekG,KumpfK,RobertsLE,ElbertT:Psychotherapyasachaoticprocess.
II.
Theapplicationsofnonlinearanalysismethodsonquasi-timeseriesoftheclient-therapistinteraction:anonstationaryapproach.
PsychotherapyResearch1997,7(3):197-218.
108.
ChamberlainL,BützMR,eds:ClinicalChaos:ATherapist'sGuidetoNonlinearDynamicsandTherapeuticChangePhiladelphia:TaylorandFrancis;1998.
109.
SchiepekG:Complexityandnonlineardynamicsinpsychotherapy.
EuropeanReview2009,17(2):331-356.
110.
PrielB,SchreiberG:Onpsychoanalysisandnon-lineardynamics.
Theparadigmofbifurcation.
BritishJournalofMedicalPsychology1994,67:209-218.
111.
TschacherW,ScheierC,GraweK:Orderandpatternformationinpsychotherapy.
NonlinearDynamics,PsychologyandLifeSciences1998,2:195-215.
112.
TschacherW,BaurN,GraweK:Temporalinteractionofprocessvariablesinpsychotherapy.
PsychotherapyResearch2000,10:296-309.
113.
NandrinoJL,LeroyF,PezardL:Dynamicsasaheuristicframeworkforpsychopathology.
ChaosandComplexityLetters2005,1(2):145-177.
114.
WaskowIE:SpecificationofthetechniquevariableintheNIMHtreatmentofdepressioncollaborativeresearchprogram.
InPsychotherapyResearch:WhereareWeandWhereShouldWeGo.
Editedby:WilliamsJBW,SpitzerRL.
NewYork:GuilfordPress;1984:150-159,1984.
115.
GranacherRP:TraumaticBrainInjury.
MethodsforClinicalandForensicNeuropsychologicalAssessment.
Secondedition.
BocaRaton,FL:CRCPress;2008.
116.
PennC,ClearyJ:Compensatorystrategiesinthelanguageofclosedheadinjuredpatients.
BrainInjury1988,2(1):3-19.
117.
SohlbergMM,MateerC:CognitiveRehabilitation.
AnIntegrativeNeuropsychologicalApproachNewYork:GuilfordPress;2001.
118.
CoelhoCA:Discourseproductiondeficitsfollowingtraumaticbraininjury:acriticalreviewoftherecentliterature.
Aphasiology1995,9:409-429.
119.
CoelhoC,YouseKM:Cognitive-communicatonrehabilitationfollowingtraumaticbraininjury.
InMedicalSpeech-LanguagePathology.
APractitioner'sGuide.
.
Secondedition.
Editedby:A.
F.
JohnsonB.
H.
Jacobson.
NewYork:ThiemeMedicalPublishers;2007:71-93.
120.
BlakemoreD:UnderstandingUtterances:ThePragmaticsofNaturalLanguageOxford:Blackwell;1990.
121.
MartinI,McDonaldS:Weakcoherence,notheoryofmindorexecutivedysfunctionSolvingtheproblemofpragmaticlanguagedisorders.
BrainandLanguage2003,85:451-466.
122.
NicholasLE,BrookshireRH:Comprehensionofspokennarrativediscoursebyadultswithaphasia,right-hemispheredamage,ortraumaticbraininjury.
AmericanJournalofSpeech-LanguagePathology1995,4:69-81.
123.
FriedlandD,MillerN:Conversationanalysisofcommunicationbreakdownafterclosedheadinjury.
BrainInjury1998,12(1):1-14.
124.
McDonaldS:Pragmaticlanguageskillsafterclosedheadinjury:Abilitytomeettheinformationalneedsofthelistener.
BrainandLanguage1993,44(1):28-46.
125.
BaltaxeCAM:Pragmaticdeficitsinthelanguageofautisticadolescents.
JournalofPediatricPsychology1977,2(4):176-180.
126.
LovelandKA,LandrySH,HughesSO,HallSK,McEvoyRE:Speechactsandthepragmaticdeficitsofautism.
JournalofSpeechandHearingResearch1988,31:593-604.
127.
CamarataSM,GibsonT:Pragmaticlanguagedeficitsinattention-deficithyperactivitydisorder.
MentalRetardationandDevelopmentalDisabilitiesResearchReviews1999,5(3):207-214.
128.
MitchellRLC,CrowTJ:Righthemispherelanguagefunctionsandschizophrenia:theforgottenhemisphere.
Brain2005,128(5):963-978.
129.
Champagne-LavauM,StipE,JoanetteY:Languagefunctionsinright-hemispheredamageandschizophrenia:apparentlysimilarpragmaticdeficitsmayhideprofounddifferences.
Brain2007,130(2):e67.
130.
BrownellH,MartinoG:Deficitsininferenceandsocialcognition:theeffectsofrighthemispheredamageondiscourse.
InRightHemisphereLanguageComprehension:PerspectivesfromCognitiveNeuroscience.
Editedby:BeemanM,ChiarelloC.
Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaumAssociates;1998:309-328.
131.
SantoroJ,SpiersM:Socialcognitivefactorsinbraininjury-associatedpersonalitychange.
BrainInjury1994,8(3):265-276.
132.
TurkstraLS,DixonTM,BakerKK:TheoryofMindandsocialbeliefsinadolescentswithtraumaticbraininjury.
Neurorehabilitation2004,19(3):245-256.
133.
BibbyH,McDonaldS:Theoryofmindaftertraumaticbraininjury.
Neuropsychologia2005,43:99-114.
134.
Havet-ThomassinV,AllainP,Etcharry-BouyxF,LeGallD:WhatabouttheoryofmindafterseverebraininjuryBrainInjury2006,20(1):83-91.
135.
HenryJD,PhillipsLH,CrawfordJR,IetswaartM,SummersF:Theoryofmindfollowingtraumaticbraininjury:theroleofemotionrecognitionandexecutivedysfunction.
Neuropsychologia2006,44(10):1623-1628.
136.
Martin-RodriquezJF,Leon-CarrionJ:Theoryofminddeficitsinpatientswithacquiredbraininjury:Aquantitativereview.
Neuropsychologia2010,48(5):1181-1191.
137.
FrithU:Autism:ExplainingtheEnigmaOxford:Blackwell;1989.
138.
JolliffeT,Baron-CohenS:Atestofcentralcoherencetheory:canadultswithhigh-functioningautismorAspergersyndromeintegratefragmentsofanobjectCognitiveNeuropsychiatry2001,6(3):193-216.
139.
HappéF,FrithU:Theweakcoherenceaccount:Detailedfocusedcognitivestyleinautismandspectrumdisorders.
JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders2006,36:5-25.
140.
GoldbergE:TheExecutiveBrain:FrontalLobesandtheCivilizedMindOxford:OxfordUniversityPress;2001.
Rappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page27of28141.
MillerEK,CohenJD:Anintegrativetheoryofprefrontalcortexfunction.
AnnualReviewsofNeuroscience2001,24:167-202.
142.
DouglasJM:Relationofexecutivefunctioningtopragmaticoutcomefollowingseveretraumaticbraininjury.
JournalofSpeech,LanguageandHearingResearch2010,53:365-382.
143.
McDonaldS:Exploringtheprocessofinferencegenerationinsarcasm:Areviewofnormalandclinicalstudies.
BrainandLanguage1999,68(3):486-506.
144.
BoothR,CharltonR,HughesC,HappéF:Disentanglingweakcoherenceandexecutivedysfunction:planningdrawinginautismandattention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder.
PhilosophicalTransactionsoftheRoyalSociety.
SeriesB.
BiologicalSciences2003,358(1430):387-392.
145.
BurnetteCP,MundyPC,MeyerJA,SuttonSK,VaughanAE,CharakD:Weakcentralcoherenceanditsrelationstotheoryofmindandanxietyinautism.
JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders2005,35(1):63-73.
146.
PellicanoE,MayberyM,MaleyA:Multiplecognitivecapabilities/deficitsinchildrenwithanautismspectrumdisorder:"weak"centralcoherenceanditsrelationtotheoryofmindandexecutivecontrol.
DevelopmentalPsychopathology2006,18(1):77-98.
147.
SnowP,DouglasJ,PonsfordJ:Conversationalassessmentfollowingtraumaticbraininjury:acomparisonacrosstwocontrolgroups.
BrainInjury1997,11(6):409-429.
148.
PellicanoE,MayberyM,MaleyA:Multiplecognitivecapabilities/deficitsinchildrenwithanautismspectrumdisorder:"weak"centralcoherenceanditsrelationtotheoryofmindandexecutivecontrol.
DevelopmentalPsychopathology2006,18(1):77-98.
149.
FriedlandD,MillerN:Conversationanalysisofcommunicationbreakdownafterclosedheadinjury.
BrainInjury1998,12(1):1-14.
150.
CoelhoCA,LilesBZ,DuffyRL,ClarksonYV:Conversationalpatternsofaphasic,closed-head-injuredandnormalspeakers.
ClinicalAphasiology1993,21:183-192.
151.
CoelhoCA,YouseKM,LeK:Conversationaldiscourseinclosed-head-injuryandnon-brain-injuredadults.
Aphasiology2002,16(4-6):659-672.
152.
DamicoJS:Clinicaldiscourseanalysis:Afunctionalapproachtolanguageassessment.
InCommunicationSkillsandClassroomSuccess.
Editedby:SimonCS.
London:TaylorandFrancis;1985:165-203.
153.
DamicoJS:Clinicaldiscourseanalysis:Afunctionalapproachtolanguageassessment.
assessmentandtherapymethodologiesforlanguageandlearningdisabledstudents.
CommunicationSkillsandClassroomSuccessEauClaire,WI:ThinkingPublications;1991,125-150.
154.
ChapmanSB:Cognitive-communicationabilitiesinchildrenwithclosedheadinjury.
AmericanJournalofSpeech-LanguagePathology1997,6(2):50-58.
155.
ChapmanSB,WatkinsR,GustafsonC,MooreS,LevinHS,KuferaJA:Narrativediscourseinchildrenwithclosedheadinjury,childrenwithlanguageimpairmentandtypicallydevelopingchildren.
AmericanJournalofSpeech-LanguagePathology1997,6(2):66-75.
156.
BodyR,ParkerM:Topicrepetitivenessaftertraumaticbraininjury:Anemergentjointlymanagedbehaviour.
ClinicalLinguisticsandPhonetics2005,19(5):379-392.
157.
MorrisJ,BleibergJ:Neuropsychologicalrehabilitationandtraditionalpsychotherapy.
InternationalJournalofClinicalNeuropsychology1986,8:133-135.
158.
JuddD,WilsonSL:Psychotherapywithbraininjurysurvivors:aninvestigationofthechallengesencounteredbycliniciansandtheirmodificationstotherapeuticpractice.
BrainInjury2005,19(6):437-449.
159.
ChaitinGJ:Informationtheoreticalcomputationalcomplexity.
IEEETransactionsonInformationTheory1974,IT20:10-15.
160.
ZvonkinAK,LevinLA:Thecomplexityoffinite-objectsandthedevelopmentoftheconceptsofinformationandrandomnessbymeansofthetheoryofalgorithms.
RussianMathemathicalSurveys1970,25:83-124.
161.
LiM,VitányiP:AnIntroductiontoKolmogorovComplexityandItsApplicationsNewYork:Springer-Verlag;1993.
162.
EbelingW,Jiménez-MontaoMA:Ongrammars,complexityandinformationmeasuresofbiologicalmacromolecules.
MathematicalBiosciences1980,52:53-71.
163.
WatanabeTAA,CellucciCJ,KohegyiE,BashoreTR,JosiassenRC,GreenbaunNN,RappPE:ThealgorithmiccomplexityofmultichannelEEGsissensitivetochangesinbehavior.
Psychophysiology2003,40:77-97.
164.
RappPE:Quantitativecharacterizationofanimalbehaviorfollowingblastexposure.
CognitiveNeurodynamics2007,1:287-293.
165.
RadhakrishnanN,WilsonD,LoizouPC:Analternativepartitioningtechniquetoquantifytheregularityofcomplextimeseries.
InternationalJournalofBifurcationandChaos10:1773-1779.
166.
BradleyPS,FayyadUM:Refininginitialpointsfork-meansclustering.
InProceedingsoftheFifteenthConferenceonMachineLearning.
Editedby:IBraskoS.
Dzeroski.
NewYork:MorganKaufman;1998:.
167.
HirataY,JuddK,KilminsterD:Estimatingageneratingpartitionfromobservedtimeseries:symbolicshadowing.
PhysicalReviewE2004,70:016215.
168.
RappPE,AlbanoAM,ZimmermanID,Jiménez-MontaoMA:Phase-randomizedsurrogatescanproducespuriousidentificationsofnon-randomstructure.
PhysicsLetters1994,192A:27-33.
169.
RappPE,CellucciCJ,KorslundKE,WatanabeTAA,Jiménez-MontaoMA:Aneffectivenormalizationofcomplexitymeasurementsforepochlengthandsamplingfrequency.
PhysicalReview2001,64E:016209-1-016209-9.
170.
RappPE,CellucciCJ,WatanabeTAA,AlbanoAM:QuantitativecharacterizationofthecomplexityofmultichannelhumanEEGs.
InternationalJournalofBifurcationandChaos2005,15:1737-1744.
171.
BarnardGA:DiscussiononthespectralanalysisofpointprocessesbyM.
S.
Bartlett.
JournaloftheRoyalStatisticalSociety1963,25B:294.
172.
HopeACA:AsimplifiedMonteCarlosignificancetestprocedure.
JournaloftheRoyalStatisticalSociety1968,30B:582-598.
173.
RsslerOE:Anequationforcontinuouschaos.
PhysicsLetters1976,57A:397-398.
174.
LambertJD:ComputationalMethodsinOrdinaryDifferentialEquationsNewYork,Wiley,NY;1976.
175.
LorenzEN:Deterministicnon-periodicflow.
JournalofAtmosphericScience1963,20:130-141.
176.
SparrowCT:TheLorenzEquationsBerlin:Springer-Verlag;1982.
177.
HénonM:Atwodimensionalmappingwithastrangeattactor.
CommunicationsinMathematicalPhysics1976,50:69-77.
178.
HitzlDH,ZeleF:AnexplorationoftheHénonquadraticmap.
PhysicaD1985,14:305-326.
179.
PressWH,FlanneryBP,TeukolskySA,VetterlingWT:NumericalRecipes.
TheArtofScientificComputingCambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress;1986.
180.
L'EcuyerP:Efficientportablecombinedrandomnumbergenerators.
CommunicaitonsoftheACM1988,31(6):742-750.
181.
BaysC,CarterSD:Improvingapoorrandomnumbergenerator.
ACMTransactionsonMathematicalSoftware1976,2(1):59-64.
Pre-publicationhistoryThepre-publicationhistoryforthispapercanbeaccessedhere:http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119/prepubdoi:10.
1186/1471-244X-11-119Citethisarticleas:Rappetal.
:Communicationpatternsinapsychotherapyfollowingtraumaticbraininjury:Aquantitativecasestudybasedonsymbolicdynamics.
BMCPsychiatry201111:119.
SubmityournextmanuscripttoBioMedCentralandtakefulladvantageof:ConvenientonlinesubmissionThoroughpeerreviewNospaceconstraintsorcolorgurechargesImmediatepublicationonacceptanceInclusioninPubMed,CAS,ScopusandGoogleScholarResearchwhichisfreelyavailableforredistributionSubmityourmanuscriptatwww.
biomedcentral.
com/submitRappetal.
BMCPsychiatry2011,11:119http://www.
biomedcentral.
com/1471-244X/11/119Page28of28
云步云怎么样?云步云是创建于2021年的品牌,主要从事出售香港vps、美国VPS、日本VPS、香港独立服务器、香港站群服务器等,机房有香港、美国、日本东京等机房,目前在售VPS线路有CN2+BGP、CN2 GIA,香港的线路也是CN2直连大陆,该公司旗下产品均采用KVM虚拟化架构。目前,云步云提供香港安畅、沙田、大浦、葵湾、将军澳、新世界等CN2机房云服务器,2核2G5M仅72.5元/月起。点击进...
Krypt这两天发布了ION平台9月份优惠信息,提供一款特选套餐年付120美元(原价$162/年),开设在洛杉矶或者圣何塞机房,支持Windows或者Linux操作系统。ion.kryptcloud.com是Krypt机房上线的云主机平台,主要提供基于KVM架构云主机产品,相对于KT主站云服务器要便宜很多,产品可选洛杉矶、圣何塞或者新加坡等地机房。洛杉矶机房CPU:2 cores内存:2GB硬盘:...
Vinahost,这个主机商还是第一次介绍到,翻看商家的介绍信息,是一家成立于2008年的老牌越南主机商,业务涵盖网站设计、域名、SSL证书、电子邮箱、虚拟主机、越南VPS、云计算、越南服务器出租以及设备托管等,机房主要在越南胡志明市的Viettle和VNPT数据中心,其中VNPT数据中心对于国内是三网直连,速度优。类似很多海外主机商一样,希望拓展自己的业务,必须要降价优惠或者增加机房迎合需求用户...
www.168dy.cc为你推荐
西部妈妈网啊,又是星期天同ip网站查询怎么查自己的服务器挂着哪些网站陈嘉垣陈浩民、马德钟强吻女星陈嘉桓,求大家一个说法。百花百游百花净斑方多少钱一盒同一服务器网站一个服务器能运行多少个网站www.kanav001.com长虹V001手机小游戏下载的网址是什么www.5any.comwww.qbo5.com 这个网站要安装播放器www.mywife.ccMywife-No 00357 MANAMI SAITO种子下载地址有么?求好心人给javbibitreebibi是什么牌子的sesehu.comwww.hu338.com 怎么看不到啊
3322动态域名注册 a2hosting 冰山互联 灵动鬼影 合租空间 卡巴斯基试用版 河南移动m值兑换 hktv in域名 超级服务器 华为云服务登录 网页提速 监控服务器 独立主机 中国联通宽带测速 域名转入 广州服务器托管 服务器防御 hdsky 腾讯服务器 更多